Conspiracy theory
A conspiracy theory is an explanation for an event or situation that asserts the existence of a
Conspiracy theories tend to be internally consistent and correlate with each other;[12] they are generally designed to resist falsification either by evidence against them or a lack of evidence for them.[13] They are reinforced by circular reasoning: both evidence against the conspiracy and absence of evidence for it are misinterpreted as evidence of its truth.[8][14] Stephan Lewandowsky observes "This interpretation relies on the notion that, the stronger the evidence against a conspiracy, the more the conspirators must want people to believe their version of events."[15] As a consequence, the conspiracy becomes a matter of faith rather than something that can be proven or disproven.[1][16] Studies have linked belief in conspiracy theories to distrust of authority and political cynicism.[17][18][19] Some researchers suggest that conspiracist ideation—belief in conspiracy theories—may be psychologically harmful or pathological.[20][21] Such belief is correlated with psychological projection, paranoia, and Machiavellianism.[22][23]
Psychologists usually attribute belief in conspiracy theories to a number of psychopathological conditions such as
Historically, conspiracy theories have been closely linked to
Conspiracy theories once limited to fringe audiences have become commonplace in
Origin and usage
The Oxford English Dictionary defines conspiracy theory as "the theory that an event or phenomenon occurs as a result of a conspiracy between interested parties; spec. a belief that some covert but influential agency (typically political in motivation and oppressive in intent) is responsible for an unexplained event". It cites a 1909 article in The American Historical Review as the earliest usage example,[52][53] although it also appeared in print for several decades before.[54]
The earliest known usage was by the American author Charles Astor Bristed, in a letter to the editor published in The New York Times on 11 January 1863.[55] He used it to refer to claims that British aristocrats were intentionally weakening the United States during the American Civil War in order to advance their financial interests.
England has had quite enough to do in Europe and Asia, without going out of her way to meddle with America. It was a physical and moral impossibility that she could be carrying on a gigantic conspiracy against us. But our masses, having only a rough general knowledge of foreign affairs, and not unnaturally somewhat exaggerating the space which we occupy in the world's eye, do not appreciate the complications which rendered such a conspiracy impossible. They only look at the sudden right-about-face movement of the English Press and public, which is most readily accounted for on the conspiracy theory.[55]
The term is also used as a way to discredit
Alleged CIA origins
The term "conspiracy theory" is itself the subject of a conspiracy theory, which posits that the term was popularized by the
Whether the CIA was responsible for popularising the term "conspiracy theory" was analyzed by Michael Butter, a Professor of American Literary and Cultural History at the University of Tübingen. Butter wrote in 2020 that the CIA document Concerning Criticism of the Warren Report, which proponents of the theory use as evidence of CIA motive and intention, does not contain the phrase "conspiracy theory" in the singular, and only uses the term "conspiracy theories" once, in the sentence: "Conspiracy theories have frequently thrown suspicion on our organisation [sic], for example, by falsely alleging that Lee Harvey Oswald worked for us."[62]
Difference from conspiracy
A conspiracy theory is not simply a
Real conspiracies, even very simple ones, are difficult to conceal and routinely experience unexpected problems.[63] In contrast, conspiracy theories suggest that conspiracies are unrealistically successful and that groups of conspirators, such as bureaucracies, can act with near-perfect competence and secrecy. The causes of events or situations are simplified to exclude complex or interacting factors, as well as the role of chance and unintended consequences. Nearly all observations are explained as having been deliberately planned by the alleged conspirators.[63]
In conspiracy theories, the conspirators are usually claimed to be acting with extreme malice.[63] As described by Robert Brotherton:
The malevolent intent assumed by most conspiracy theories goes far beyond everyday plots borne out of self-interest, corruption, cruelty, and criminality. The postulated conspirators are not merely people with selfish agendas or differing values. Rather, conspiracy theories postulate a black-and-white world in which good is struggling against evil. The general public is cast as the victim of organised persecution, and the motives of the alleged conspirators often verge on pure maniacal evil. At the very least, the conspirators are said to have an almost inhuman disregard for the basic liberty and well-being of the general population. More grandiose conspiracy theories portray the conspirators as being Evil Incarnate: of having caused all the ills from which we suffer, committing abominable acts of unthinkable cruelty on a routine basis, and striving ultimately to subvert or destroy everything we hold dear.[63]
Examples
A conspiracy theory may take any matter as its subject, but certain subjects attract greater interest than others. Favored subjects include famous deaths and assassinations, morally dubious government activities, suppressed technologies, and "false flag" terrorism. Among the longest-standing and most widely recognized conspiracy theories are notions concerning the assassination of John F. Kennedy, the 1969 Apollo Moon landings, and the 9/11 terrorist attacks, as well as numerous theories pertaining to alleged plots for world domination by various groups, both real and imaginary.[64]
Popularity
Conspiracy beliefs are widespread around the world.
Conspiracy theories once limited to fringe audiences have become commonplace in
Conspiracy theories are often commonly believed, some even being held by the majority of the population.[48][49][50] A broad cross-section of Americans today gives credence to at least some conspiracy theories.[67] For instance, a study conducted in 2016 found that 10% of Americans think the chemtrail conspiracy theory is "completely true" and 20–30% think it is "somewhat true".[68] This puts "the equivalent of 120 million Americans in the 'chemtrails are real' camp".[68] Belief in conspiracy theories has therefore become a topic of interest for sociologists, psychologists and experts in folklore.
Conspiracy theories are widely present on the Web in the form of blogs and YouTube videos, as well as on social media. Whether the Web has increased the prevalence of conspiracy theories or not is an open research question.[69] The presence and representation of conspiracy theories in search engine results has been monitored and studied, showing significant variation across different topics, and a general absence of reputable, high-quality links in the results.[70]
One conspiracy theory that propagated through former US President Barack Obama's time in office[71] claimed that he was born in Kenya, instead of Hawaii where he was actually born.[72] Former governor of Arkansas and political opponent of Obama Mike Huckabee made headlines in 2011[73] when he, among other members of Republican leadership, continued to question Obama's citizenship status.
Conspiracy theory | Believe | Not sure |
---|---|---|
"A group of Satan-worshipping elites who run a child sex ring are trying to control our politics and media" (QAnon) | 17%
|
37%
|
"Several mass shootings in recent years were staged hoaxes" (crisis actor theory) | 12%
|
27%
|
birtherism )
|
19%
|
22%
|
Moon landing conspiracy theories | 8%
|
20%
|
9/11 conspiracy theories | 7%
|
20%
|
Types
A conspiracy theory can be local or international, focused on single events or covering multiple incidents and entire countries, regions and periods of history.
Walker's five kinds
Jesse Walker (2013) has identified five kinds of conspiracy theories:[77]
- The "Enemy Outside" refers to theories based on figures alleged to be scheming against a community from without.
- The "Enemy Within" finds the conspirators lurking inside the nation, indistinguishable from ordinary citizens.
- The "Enemy Above" involves powerful people manipulating events for their own gain.
- The "Enemy Below" features the lower classes working to overturn the social order.
- The "Benevolent Conspiracies" are angelic forces that work behind the scenes to improve the world and help people.
Barkun's three types
Michael Barkun has identified three classifications of conspiracy theory:[78]
- Event conspiracy theories. This refers to limited and well-defined events. Examples may include such conspiracies theories as those concerning the spread of AIDS.
- Systemic conspiracy theories. The conspiracy is believed to have broad goals, usually conceived as securing control of a country, a region, or even the entire world. The goals are sweeping, whilst the conspiratorial machinery is generally simple: a single, evil organization implements a plan to infiltrate and subvert existing institutions. This is a common scenario in conspiracy theories that focus on the alleged machinations of Jews, Freemasons, Communism, or the Catholic Church.
- Superconspiracy theories. For Barkun, such theories link multiple alleged conspiracies together hierarchically. At the summit is a distant but all-powerful evil force. His cited examples are the ideas of David Icke and Milton William Cooper.
Rothbard: shallow vs. deep
Lack of evidence
Belief in conspiracy theories is generally based not on evidence, but in the faith of the believer.[80] Noam Chomsky contrasts conspiracy theory to institutional analysis which focuses mostly on the public, long-term behavior of publicly known institutions, as recorded in, for example, scholarly documents or mainstream media reports.[81] Conspiracy theory conversely posits the existence of secretive coalitions of individuals and speculates on their alleged activities.[82][83] Belief in conspiracy theories is associated with biases in reasoning, such as the conjunction fallacy.[84]
Clare Birchall at King's College London describes conspiracy theory as a "form of popular knowledge or interpretation".[a] The use of the word 'knowledge' here suggests ways in which conspiracy theory may be considered in relation to legitimate modes of knowing.[b] The relationship between legitimate and illegitimate knowledge, Birchall claims, is closer than common dismissals of conspiracy theory contend.[86]
Theories involving multiple conspirators that are proven to be correct, such as the
Rhetoric
Conspiracy theory rhetoric exploits several important cognitive biases, including proportionality bias, attribution bias, and confirmation bias.[33] Their arguments often take the form of asking reasonable questions, but without providing an answer based on strong evidence.[91] Conspiracy theories are most successful when proponents can gather followers from the general public, such as in politics, religion and journalism. These proponents may not necessarily believe the conspiracy theory; instead, they may just use it in an attempt to gain public approval. Conspiratorial claims can act as a successful rhetorical strategy to convince a portion of the public via appeal to emotion.[29]
Conspiracy theories typically justify themselves by focusing on gaps or ambiguities in knowledge, and then arguing that the true explanation for this must be a conspiracy.[63] In contrast, any evidence that directly supports their claims is generally of low quality. For example, conspiracy theories are often dependent on eyewitness testimony, despite its unreliability, while disregarding objective analyses of the evidence.[63]
Conspiracy theories are not able to be
Conspiracy theorists often take advantage of false balance in the media. They may claim to be presenting a legitimate alternative viewpoint that deserves equal time to argue its case; for example, this strategy has been used by the Teach the Controversy campaign to promote intelligent design, which often claims that there is a conspiracy of scientists suppressing their views. If they successfully find a platform to present their views in a debate format, they focus on using rhetorical ad hominems and attacking perceived flaws in the mainstream account, while avoiding any discussion of the shortcomings in their own position.[29]
The typical approach of conspiracy theories is to challenge any action or statement from authorities, using even the most tenuous justifications. Responses are then assessed using a double standard, where failing to provide an immediate response to the satisfaction of the conspiracy theorist will be claimed to prove a conspiracy. Any minor errors in the response are heavily emphasized, while deficiencies in the arguments of other proponents are generally excused.[29]
In science, conspiracists may suggest that a scientific theory can be disproven by a single perceived deficiency, even though such events are extremely rare. In addition, both disregarding the claims and attempting to address them will be interpreted as proof of a conspiracy.[29] Other conspiracist arguments may not be scientific; for example, in response to the IPCC Second Assessment Report in 1996, much of the opposition centered on promoting a procedural objection to the report's creation. Specifically, it was claimed that part of the procedure reflected a conspiracy to silence dissenters, which served as motivation for opponents of the report and successfully redirected a significant amount of the public discussion away from the science.[29]
Consequences
Historically, conspiracy theories have been closely linked to
Conspiracy theories are a significant obstacle to improvements in
Conspiracy theories are a fundamental component of a wide range of radicalized and extremist groups, where they may play an important role in reinforcing the ideology and psychology of their members as well as further radicalizing their beliefs.
Conspiracy theorizing in the workplace can also have economic consequences. For example, it leads to lower job satisfaction and lower commitment, resulting in workers being more likely to leave their jobs.[28] Comparisons have also been made with the effects of workplace rumors, which share some characteristics with conspiracy theories and result in both decreased productivity and increased stress. Subsequent effects on managers include reduced profits, reduced trust from employees, and damage to the company's image.[28][93]
Conspiracy theories can divert attention from important social, political, and scientific issues.[94][95] In addition, they have been used to discredit scientific evidence to the general public or in a legal context. Conspiratorial strategies also share characteristics with those used by lawyers who are attempting to discredit expert testimony, such as claiming that the experts have ulterior motives in testifying, or attempting to find someone who will provide statements to imply that expert opinion is more divided than it actually is.[29]
It is possible that conspiracy theories may also produce some compensatory benefits to society in certain situations. For example, they may help people identify governmental deceptions, particularly in repressive societies, and encourage
Interventions
Target audience
Strategies to address conspiracy theories have been divided into two categories based on whether the target audience is the conspiracy theorists or the general public.[51][49] These strategies have been described as reducing either the supply or the demand for conspiracy theories.[49] Both approaches can be used at the same time, although there may be issues of limited resources, or if arguments are used which may appeal to one audience at the expense of the other.[49]
Brief scientific literacy interventions, particularly those focusing on critical thinking skills, can effectively undermine conspiracy beliefs and related behaviors. Research led by Penn State scholars, published in the Journal of Consumer Research, found that enhancing scientific knowledge and reasoning through short interventions, such as videos explaining concepts like correlation and causation, reduces the endorsement of conspiracy theories. These interventions were most effective against conspiracy theories based on faulty reasoning and were successful even among groups prone to conspiracy beliefs. The studies, involving over 2,700 participants, highlight the importance of educational interventions in mitigating conspiracy beliefs, especially when timed to influence critical decision-making.[97][98]
General public
People who feel
Methods of refutation which have shown effectiveness in various circumstances include: providing facts that demonstrate the conspiracy theory is false, attempting to discredit the source, explaining how the logic is invalid or misleading, and providing links to fact-checking websites.[51] It can also be effective to use these strategies in advance, informing people that they could encounter misleading information in the future, and why the information should be rejected (also called inoculation or prebunking).[51][99][100] While it has been suggested that discussing conspiracy theories can raise their profile and make them seem more legitimate to the public, the discussion can put people on guard instead as long as it is sufficiently persuasive.[9]
Other approaches to reduce the appeal of conspiracy theories in general among the public may be based in the emotional and social nature of conspiratorial beliefs. For example, interventions that promote
Conspiracy theorists
It is much more difficult to convince people who already believe in conspiracy theories.
Characteristics of successful strategies for reaching conspiracy theorists have been divided into several broad categories: 1) Arguments can be presented by "trusted messengers", such as people who were formerly members of an extremist group. 2) Since conspiracy theorists think of themselves as people who value critical thinking, this can be affirmed and then redirected to encourage being more critical when analyzing the conspiracy theory. 3) Approaches demonstrate empathy, and are based on building understanding together, which is supported by modeling open-mindedness in order to encourage the conspiracy theorists to do likewise. 4) The conspiracy theories are not attacked with ridicule or aggressive deconstruction, and interactions are not treated like an argument to be won; this approach can work with the general public, but among conspiracy theorists it may simply be rejected.[51]
Interventions that reduce feelings of uncertainty, anxiety, or powerlessness result in a reduction in conspiracy beliefs.[42] Other possible strategies to mitigate the effect of conspiracy theories include education, media literacy, and increasing governmental openness and transparency.[99] Due to the relationship between conspiracy theories and political extremism, the academic literature on deradicalization is also important.[51]
One approach describes conspiracy theories as resulting from a "crippled epistemology", in which a person encounters or accepts very few relevant sources of information.[49][101] A conspiracy theory is more likely to appear justified to people with a limited "informational environment" who only encounter misleading information. These people may be "epistemologically isolated" in self-enclosed networks. From the perspective of people within these networks, disconnected from the information available to the rest of society, believing in conspiracy theories may appear to be justified.[49][101] In these cases, the solution would be to break the group's informational isolation.[49]
Reducing transmission
Public exposure to conspiracy theories can be reduced by interventions that reduce their ability to spread, such as by encouraging people to reflect before sharing a news story.[51] Researchers Carlos Diaz Ruiz and Tomas Nilsson have proposed technical and rhetorical interventions to counter the spread of conspiracy theories on social media.[102]
Type of intervention | Intervention |
---|---|
Technical | Expose sources that insert and circulate conspiracy theories on social media (flagging). |
Diminish the source's capacity to monetize conspiracies (demonetization). | |
Slow down the circulation of conspiracy theories (algorithm) | |
Rhetorical | Issue authoritative corrections (fact-checking). |
Authority-based corrections and fact-checking may backfire because personal worldviews cannot be proved wrong. | |
Enlist spokespeople that can be perceived as allies and insiders. | |
Rebuttals must spring from an epistemology that participants are already familiar with. | |
Give believers of conspiracies an "exit ramp" to dis-invest themselves without facing ridicule. |
Government policies
The primary defense against conspiracy theories is to maintain an
Joseph Pierre has said that mistrust in authoritative institutions is the core component underlying many conspiracy theories and that this mistrust creates an epistemic vacuum and makes individuals searching for answers vulnerable to misinformation. Therefore, one possible solution is offering consumers a seat at the table to mend their mistrust in institutions.[103] Regarding the challenges of this approach, Pierre has said, "The challenge with acknowledging areas of uncertainty within a public sphere is that doing so can be weaponized to reinforce a post-truth view of the world in which everything is debatable, and any counter-position is just as valid. Although I like to think of myself as a middle of the road kind of individual, it is important to keep in mind that the truth does not always lie in the middle of a debate, whether we are talking about climate change, vaccines, or antipsychotic medications."[104]
Researchers have recommended that public policies should take into account the possibility of conspiracy theories relating to any policy or policy area, and prepare to combat them in advance.
In addition, when the government communicates with citizens to combat conspiracy theories, online methods are more efficient compared to other methods such as print publications. This also promotes transparency, can improve a message's perceived trustworthiness, and is more effective at reaching underrepresented demographics. However, as of 2019[update], many governmental websites do not take full advantage of the available information-sharing opportunities. Similarly, social media accounts need to be used effectively in order to achieve meaningful communication with the public, such as by responding to requests that citizens send to those accounts. Other steps include adapting messages to the communication styles used on the social media platform in question, and promoting a culture of openness. Since mixed messaging can support conspiracy theories, it is also important to avoid conflicting accounts, such as by ensuring the accuracy of messages on the social media accounts of individual members of the organization.[99]
Public health campaigns
Successful methods for dispelling conspiracy theories have been studied in the context of public health campaigns. A key characteristic of communication strategies to address medical conspiracy theories is the use of techniques that rely less on emotional appeals. It is more effective to use methods that encourage people to process information rationally. The use of visual aids is also an essential part of these strategies. Since conspiracy theories are based on intuitive thinking, and visual information processing relies on intuition, visual aids are able to compete directly for the public's attention.[9]
In public health campaigns, information retention by the public is highest for loss-framed messages that include more extreme outcomes. However, excessively appealing to catastrophic scenarios (e.g. low vaccination rates causing an epidemic) may provoke anxiety, which is associated with conspiracism and could increase belief in conspiracy theories instead.
A particularly important technique is the use of focus groups to understand exactly what people believe, and the reasons they give for those beliefs. This allows messaging to focus on the specific concerns that people identify, and on topics that are easily misinterpreted by the public, since these are factors which conspiracy theories can take advantage of. In addition, discussions with focus groups and observations of the group dynamics can indicate which anti-conspiracist ideas are most likely to spread.[9]
Interventions that address medical conspiracy theories by reducing powerlessness include emphasizing the principle of informed consent, giving patients all the relevant information without imposing decisions on them, to ensure that they have a sense of control. Improving access to healthcare also reduces medical conspiracism. However, doing so by political efforts can also fuel additional conspiracy theories, which occurred with the Affordable Care Act (Obamacare) in the United States. Another successful strategy is to require people to watch a short video when they fulfil requirements such as registration for school or a drivers' license, which has been demonstrated to improve vaccination rates and signups for organ donation.[9]
Another approach is based on viewing conspiracy theories as narratives which express personal and cultural values, making them less susceptible to straightforward factual corrections, and more effectively addressed by counter-narratives.[100][105] Counter-narratives can be more engaging and memorable than simple corrections, and can be adapted to the specific values held by individuals and cultures. These narratives may depict personal experiences, or alternatively they can be cultural narratives. In the context of vaccination, examples of cultural narratives include stories about scientific breakthroughs, about the world before vaccinations, or about heroic and altruistic researchers. The themes to be addressed would be those that could be exploited by conspiracy theories to increase vaccine hesitancy, such as perceptions of vaccine risk, lack of patient empowerment, and lack of trust in medical authorities.[100]
Backfire effects
It has been suggested that directly countering misinformation can be counterproductive. For example, since conspiracy theories can reinterpret disconfirming information as part of their narrative, refuting a claim can result in accidentally reinforcing it,[63][106] which is referred to as a "backfire effect".[107] In addition, publishing criticism of conspiracy theories can result in legitimizing them.[94] In this context, possible interventions include carefully selecting which conspiracy theories to refute, requesting additional analyses from independent observers, and introducing cognitive diversity into conspiratorial communities by undermining their poor epistemology.[94] Any legitimization effect might also be reduced by responding to more conspiracy theories rather than fewer.[49]
There are psychological mechanisms by which backfire effects could potentially occur, but the evidence on this topic is mixed, and backfire effects are very rare in practice.[100][107][108] A 2020 review of the scientific literature on backfire effects found that there have been widespread failures to replicate their existence, even under conditions that would be theoretically favorable to observing them.[107] Due to the lack of reproducibility, as of 2020[update] most researchers believe that backfire effects are either unlikely to occur on the broader population level, or they only occur in very specific circumstances, or they do not exist.[107] Brendan Nyhan, one of the researchers who initially proposed the occurrence of backfire effects, wrote in 2021 that the persistence of misinformation is most likely due to other factors.[108]
In general, people do reject conspiracy theories when they learn about their contradictions and lack of evidence.[9] For most people, corrections and fact-checking are very unlikely to have a negative impact, and there is no specific group of people in which backfire effects have been consistently observed.[107] Presenting people with factual corrections, or highlighting the logical contradictions in conspiracy theories, has been demonstrated to have a positive effect in many circumstances.[48][106] For example, this has been studied in the case of informing believers in 9/11 conspiracy theories about statements by actual experts and witnesses.[48] One possibility is that criticism is most likely to backfire if it challenges someone's worldview or identity. This suggests that an effective approach may be to provide criticism while avoiding such challenges.[106]
Psychology
The widespread belief in conspiracy theories has become a topic of interest for sociologists, psychologists, and experts in folklore since at least the 1960s, when
Conspiracism serves the needs of diverse political and social groups in America and elsewhere. It identifies elites, blames them for economic and social catastrophes, and assumes that things will be better once popular action can remove them from positions of power. As such, conspiracy theories do not typify a particular epoch or ideology.[111]: 199
Research suggests, on a psychological level, conspiracist ideation—belief in conspiracy theories—can be harmful or pathological,
Conspiracy theories often make use of complicated and detailed arguments, including ones which appear to be analytical or scientific. However, belief in conspiracy theories is primarily driven by emotion.
Some psychological scientists assert that explanations related to conspiracy theories can be, and often are "internally consistent" with strong beliefs that had previously been held prior to the event that sparked the conspiracy.[42] People who believe in conspiracy theories tend to believe in other unsubstantiated claims – including pseudoscience and paranormal phenomena.[121]
Attractions
Psychological motives for believing in conspiracy theories can be categorized as epistemic, existential, or social. These motives are particularly acute in vulnerable and disadvantaged populations. However, it does not appear that the beliefs help to address these motives; in fact, they may be self-defeating, acting to make the situation worse instead.[42][106] For example, while conspiratorial beliefs can result from a perceived sense of powerlessness, exposure to conspiracy theories immediately suppresses personal feelings of autonomy and control. Furthermore, they also make people less likely to take actions that could improve their circumstances.[42][106]
This is additionally supported by the fact that conspiracy theories have a number of disadvantageous attributes.[42] For example, they promote a negative and distrustful view of other people and groups, who are allegedly acting based on antisocial and cynical motivations. This is expected to lead to increased alienation and anomie, and reduced social capital. Similarly, they depict the public as ignorant and powerless against the alleged conspirators, with important aspects of society determined by malevolent forces, a viewpoint which is likely to be disempowering.[42]
Each person may endorse conspiracy theories for one of many different reasons.
The political scientist Michael Barkun, discussing the usage of "conspiracy theory" in contemporary American culture, holds that this term is used for a belief that explains an event as the result of a secret plot by exceptionally powerful and cunning conspirators to achieve a malevolent end.[123][124] According to Barkun, the appeal of conspiracism is threefold:
- First, conspiracy theories claim to explain what institutional analysis cannot. They appear to make sense out of a world that is otherwise confusing.
- Second, they do so in an appealingly simple way, by dividing the world sharply between the forces of light, and the forces of darkness. They trace all evil back to a single source, the conspirators and their agents.
- Third, conspiracy theories are often presented as special,
brainwashed herd, while the conspiracy theorists in the know can congratulate themselves on penetrating the plotters' deceptions.[124]
This third point is supported by research of Roland Imhoff, professor of
People formulate conspiracy theories to explain, for example, power relations in social groups and the perceived existence of evil forces.[c][124][109][110] Proposed psychological origins of conspiracy theorising include projection; the personal need to explain "a significant event [with] a significant cause;" and the product of various kinds and stages of thought disorder, such as paranoid disposition, ranging in severity to diagnosable mental illnesses. Some people prefer socio-political explanations over the insecurity of encountering random, unpredictable, or otherwise inexplicable events.[127][128][129][130][131][132] According to Berlet and Lyons, "Conspiracism is a particular narrative form of scapegoating that frames demonized enemies as part of a vast insidious plot against the common good, while it valorizes the scapegoater as a hero for sounding the alarm".[133]
Causes
Some psychologists believe that a search for meaning is common in conspiracism. Once cognized, confirmation bias and avoidance of cognitive dissonance may reinforce the belief. In a context where a conspiracy theory has become embedded within a social group, communal reinforcement may also play a part.[134]
Inquiry into possible motives behind the accepting of irrational conspiracy theories has linked
Professor
Some researchers have suggested that conspiracy theories could be partially caused by psychological mechanisms the human brain possesses for detecting dangerous coalitions. Such a mechanism could have been useful in the small-scale environment humanity evolved in but are mismatched in a modern, complex society and thus "misfire", perceiving conspiracies where none exist.[141]
Projection
Some historians have argued that psychological projection is prevalent amongst conspiracy theorists. This projection, according to the argument, is manifested in the form of attribution of undesirable characteristics of the self to the conspirators. Historian Richard Hofstadter stated that:
This enemy seems on many counts a projection of the self; both the ideal and the unacceptable aspects of the self are attributed to him. A fundamental paradox of the paranoid style is the imitation of the enemy. The enemy, for example, may be the cosmopolitan intellectual, but the paranoid will outdo him in the apparatus of scholarship, even of pedantry. ... The Ku Klux Klan imitated Catholicism to the point of donning priestly vestments, developing an elaborate ritual and an equally elaborate hierarchy. The John Birch Society emulates Communist cells and quasi-secret operation through "front" groups, and preaches a ruthless prosecution of the ideological war along lines very similar to those it finds in the Communist enemy. Spokesmen of the various fundamentalist anti-Communist "crusades" openly express their admiration for the dedication, discipline, and strategic ingenuity the Communist cause calls forth.[130]
Hofstadter also noted that "sexual freedom" is a vice frequently attributed to the conspiracist's target group, noting that "very often the fantasies of true believers reveal strong sadomasochistic outlets, vividly expressed, for example, in the delight of anti-Masons with the cruelty of Masonic punishments".[130]
Physiology
Marcel Danesi suggests that people who believe conspiracy theories have difficulty rethinking situations. Exposure to those theories has caused neural pathways which are more rigid and less subject to change. Initial susceptibility to believing the lies, dehumanizing language, and metaphors of these theories leads to the acceptance of larger and more extensive theories because the hardened neural pathways are already present. Repetition of the "facts" of conspiracy theories and their connected lies simply reinforces the rigidity of those pathways. Thus, conspiracy theories and dehumanizing lies are not mere hyperbole, they can actually change the way people think:
Unfortunately, research into this brain wiring also shows that once people begin to believe lies, they are unlikely to change their minds even when confronted with evidence that contradicts their beliefs. It is a form of brainwashing. Once the brain has carved out a well-worn path of believing deceit, it is even harder to step out of that path – which is how fanatics are born. Instead, these people will seek out information that confirms their beliefs, avoid anything that is in conflict with them, or even turn the contrasting information on its head, so as to make it fit their beliefs.
People with strong convictions will have a hard time changing their minds, given how embedded a lie becomes in the mind. In fact, there are scientists and scholars still studying the best tools and tricks to combat lies with some combination of brain training and linguistic awareness.[142]
Sociology
In addition to psychological factors such as conspiracist ideation, sociological factors also help account for who believes in which conspiracy theories. Such theories tend to get more traction among election losers in society, for example, and the emphasis of conspiracy theories by elites and leaders tends to increase belief among followers who have higher levels of conspiracy thinking.[143] Christopher Hitchens described conspiracy theories as the "exhaust fumes of democracy":[131] the unavoidable result of a large amount of information circulating among a large number of people.
Conspiracy theories may be emotionally satisfying, by assigning blame to a group to which the theorist does not belong and so absolving the theorist of moral or political responsibility in society.[144] Likewise, Roger Cohen writing for The New York Times has said that, "captive minds; ... resort to conspiracy theory because it is the ultimate refuge of the powerless. If you cannot change your own life, it must be that some greater force controls the world."[132]
Sociological historian Holger Herwig found in studying German explanations for the origins of World War I, "Those events that are most important are hardest to understand because they attract the greatest attention from myth makers and charlatans."[145] Justin Fox of Time magazine argues that Wall Street traders are among the most conspiracy-minded group of people, and ascribes this to the reality of some financial market conspiracies, and to the ability of conspiracy theories to provide necessary orientation in the market's day-to-day movements.[127]
Influence of critical theory
Fusion paranoia
Barkun has adopted this term to refer to how the synthesis of paranoid conspiracy theories, which were once limited to American fringe audiences, has given them mass appeal and enabled them to become commonplace in mass media,[148] thereby inaugurating an unrivaled period of people actively preparing for apocalyptic or millenarian scenarios in the United States of the late 20th and early 21st centuries.[149] Barkun notes the occurrence of lone-wolf conflicts with law enforcement acting as proxy for threatening the established political powers.[150]
Viability
As evidence that undermines an alleged conspiracy grows, the number of alleged conspirators also grows in the minds of conspiracy theorists. This is because of an assumption that the alleged conspirators often have competing interests. For example, if Republican President George W. Bush is allegedly responsible for the 9/11 terrorist attacks, and the Democratic party did not pursue exposing this alleged plot, that must mean that both the Democratic and Republican parties are conspirators in the alleged plot. It also assumes that the alleged conspirators are so competent that they can fool the entire world, but so incompetent that even the unskilled conspiracy theorists can find mistakes they make that prove the fraud. At some point, the number of alleged conspirators, combined with the contradictions within the alleged conspirators' interests and competence, becomes so great that maintaining the theory becomes an obvious exercise in absurdity.[151]
The physicist
- A Moon landing hoax would require the involvement of 411,000 people and would be exposed within 3.68 years;
- Climate-change fraudwould require a minimum of 29,083 people (published climate scientists only) and would be exposed within 26.77 years, or up to 405,000 people, in which case it would be exposed within 3.70 years;
- A vaccination conspiracy would require a minimum of 22,000 people (without drug companies) and would be exposed within at least 3.15 years and at most 34.78 years depending on the number involved;
- A conspiracy to suppress a cure for cancerwould require 714,000 people and would be exposed within 3.17 years.
Grimes's study did not consider exposure by sources outside of the alleged conspiracy. It only considered exposure from within the alleged conspiracy through whistleblowers or through incompetence.[154] Subsequent comments on the PubPeer website[155] point out that these calculations must exclude successful conspiracies since, by definition, we don't know about them, and are wrong by an order of magnitude about Bletchley Park, which remained a secret far longer than Grimes' calculations predicted.
Terminology
The term "truth seeker" is adopted by some conspiracy theorists when describing themselves on social media.
Politics
The philosopher Karl Popper described the central problem of conspiracy theories as a form of fundamental attribution error, where every event is generally perceived as being intentional and planned, greatly underestimating the effects of randomness and unintended consequences.[95] In his book The Open Society and Its Enemies, he used the term "the conspiracy theory of society" to denote the idea that social phenomena such as "war, unemployment, poverty, shortages ... [are] the result of direct design by some powerful individuals and groups".[161] Popper argued that totalitarianism was founded on conspiracy theories which drew on imaginary plots which were driven by paranoid scenarios predicated on tribalism, chauvinism, or racism. He also noted that conspirators very rarely achieved their goal.[162]
Historically, real conspiracies have usually had little effect on history and have had
Arab world
Conspiracy theories are a prevalent feature of
Turkey
Conspiracy theories are a prevalent feature of culture and politics in Turkey. Conspiracism is an important phenomenon in understanding Turkish politics.[171] This is explained by a desire to "make up for our lost Ottoman grandeur",[171] the humiliation of perceiving Turkey as part of "the malfunctioning half" of the world,[172] and a "low level of media literacy among the Turkish population."[173]
There are a wide variety of conspiracy theories including the
United States
The historian Richard Hofstadter addressed the role of paranoia and conspiracism throughout U.S. history in his 1964 essay "The Paranoid Style in American Politics". Bernard Bailyn's classic The Ideological Origins of the American Revolution (1967) notes that a similar phenomenon could be found in North America during the time preceding the American Revolution. Conspiracism labels people's attitudes as well as the type of conspiracy theories that are more global and historical in proportion.[179]
Harry G. West and others have noted that while conspiracy theorists may often be dismissed as a fringe minority, certain evidence suggests that a wide range of the U.S. maintains a belief in conspiracy theories. West also compares those theories to
In his studies of "American political demonology", historian
Historian Kathryn S. Olmsted cites three reasons why Americans are prone to believing in government conspiracies theories:
- Genuine government overreach and secrecy during the Cold War, such as assassination attempts on Fidel Castroin collaboration with mobsters.
- Precedent set by official government-sanctioned conspiracy theories for propaganda, such as claims of German infiltration of the U.S. during World War II or the debunked claim that Saddam Hussein played a role in the 9/11 attacks.
- Distrust fostered by the government's spying on and harassment of dissenters, such as the Sedition Act of 1918, COINTELPRO, and as part of various Red Scares.[184]
See also
- Big lie – Propaganda technique
- Brainwashing – Concept that the human mind can be altered or controlled
- Cherry picking – Fallacy of incomplete evidence
- Conspiracy fiction – Subgenre of thriller fiction
- Fake news – False or misleading information presented as real
- Fringe theory – Idea which departs from accepted scholarship in the field
- Furtive fallacy – Informal fallacy of emphasis
- Hanlon's razor – Adage to assume stupidity over malice
- Influencing machine – 1919 article in psychoanalysis
- List of conspiracy theories
- List of fallacies
- List of topics characterized as pseudoscience
- Occam's razor – Philosophical problem-solving principle
- Philosophy of conspiracy theories – Branch of academic study
- Propaganda – Communication used to influence opinion
- Pseudohistory – Pseudoscholarship that attempts to distort historical record
- Pseudoscience – Unscientific claims wrongly presented as scientific
- Superstition – Belief or behavior that is considered irrational or supernatural
References
Informational notes
- ^ Birchall 2006: "[W]e can appreciate conspiracy theory as a unique form of popular knowledge or interpretation, and address what this might mean for any knowledge we produce about it or how we interpret it."[85]: 66
- ^ Birchall 2006: "What we quickly discover ... is that it becomes impossible to map conspiracy theory and academic discourse onto a clear illegitimate/legitimate divide."[85]: 72
- ^ Barkun 2003: "The essence of conspiracy beliefs lies in attempts to delineate and explain evil. At their broadest, conspiracy theories 'view history as controlled by massive, demonic forces.' ... For our purposes, a conspiracy belief is the belief that an organization made up of individuals or groups was or is acting covertly to achieve a malevolent end."[123]
Citations
- ^ a b c Barkun, Michael (2003). A Culture of Conspiracy: Apocalyptic Visions in Contemporary America. Berkeley: University of California Press. pp. 3–4.
- ISBN 978-1-61069-478-0.
- ^ .
- JSTOR 3791630.
explanations for important events that involve secret plots by powerful and malevolent groups
- ^ "conspiracy theory". Oxford English Dictionary (Online ed.). Oxford University Press. (Subscription or participating institution membership required.) "the theory that an event or phenomenon occurs as a result of a conspiracy between interested parties; spec. a belief that some covert but influential agency (typically political in motivation and oppressive in intent) is responsible for an unexplained event"
- S2CID 16685781.
A conspiracist belief can be described as 'the unnecessary assumption of conspiracy when other explanations are more probable'.
- ^ Additional sources:
- Aaronovitch, David (2009). Voodoo Histories: The Role of the Conspiracy Theory in Shaping Modern History. Jonathan Cape. p. 253. ISBN 9780224074704. Retrieved 17 August 2019.
It is a contention of this book that conspiracy theorists fail to apply the principle of Occam's razor to their arguments.
- Brotherton, Robert; French, Christopher C. (2014). "Belief in Conspiracy Theories and Susceptibility to the Conjunction Fallacy". Applied Cognitive Psychology. 28 (2): 238–248. doi:10.1002/acp.2995
A conspiracy theory can be defined as an unverified and relatively implausible allegation of conspiracy, claiming that significant events are the result of a secret plot carried out by a preternaturally sinister and powerful group of people.
- Aaronovitch, David (2009). Voodoo Histories: The Role of the Conspiracy Theory in Shaping Modern History. Jonathan Cape. p. 253.
- Jonason, Peter Karl; March, Evita; Springer, Jordan (2019). "Belief in conspiracy theories: The predictive role of schizotypy, Machiavellianism, and primary psychopathy". PLOS ONE. 14 (12): e0225964. PMID 31794581.
Conspiracy theories are a subset of false beliefs, and generally implicate a malevolent force (e.g., a government body or secret society) involved in orchestrating major events or providing misinformation regarding the details of events to an unwitting public, in part of a plot towards achieving a sinister goal.
- Thresher-Andrews, Christopher (2013). "An introduction into the world of conspiracy" (PDF). PsyPAG Quarterly. 1 (88): 5–8. S2CID 255932379.
Conspiracy theories are unsubstantiated, less plausible alternatives to the mainstream explanation of the event; they assume everything is intended, with malignity. Crucially, they are also epistemically self-insulating in their construction and arguments.
- ^ OCLC 802867724.
- ^ (PDF) from the original on 8 May 2020. Retrieved 7 October 2021.
- ^ S2CID 152217672.
- ^ S2CID 141788005. Archived from the original(PDF) on 7 October 2013.
A conspiracy theory is not merely one candidate explanation among other equally plausible alternatives. Rather, the label refers to a claim which runs counter to a more plausible and widely accepted account...[Conspiratorial beliefs are] invariably at odds with the mainstream consensus among scientists, historians, or other legitimate judges of the claim's veracity.
- ^ S2CID 252597317.
- (PDF) from the original on 3 November 2018. Retrieved 8 May 2024.
- ^ JSTOR 2564659.
- PMID 24098391.
- ^ a b Barkun, Michael (2011). Chasing Phantoms: Reality, Imagination, and Homeland Security Since 9/11. Chapel Hill: University of North Carolina Press. p. 10.
- PMID 22888323.
- ISBN 978-0-19-757353-2, retrieved 17 May 2022
- PMID 24586574.
- ^ PMID 28352955.
- ^ .
- (PDF) from the original on 3 November 2018. Retrieved 28 December 2018.
- PMID 20402445.
- ^ Dean, Signe (23 October 2017). "Conspiracy Theorists Really Do See The World Differently, New Study Shows". Science Alert. Retrieved 17 June 2020.
- ^ Sloat, Sarah (17 October 2017). "Conspiracy Theorists Have a Fundamental Cognitive Problem, Say Scientists". Inverse. Retrieved 17 June 2020.
- PMID 33867616.
- PMID 29081831.
- ^ hdl:10261/369501.
- ^ PMID 20539311.
- S2CID 233429880.
- S2CID 158560266.
Conspiracy theories often function as popular conduits of ethno-religious hatred and conflict.
- ^ Göknar, Erdağ (2019). "Conspiracy Theory in Turkey: Politics and Protest in the Age of "Post-Truth" by Julian de Medeiros (review)". The Middle East Journal. 73 (2): 336–337.
- ^ S2CID 255932379.
- ^ S2CID 23483038.
- ^ PMID 27512452.
- ^ (PDF) from the original on 27 April 2021. Retrieved 7 October 2021.
- ^ S2CID 237329082.
- ^ "QAnon reshaped Trump's party and radicalized believers. The Capitol siege may just be the start". The Washington Post. 13 January 2021.
- ^ ISBN 978-1-84593-204-6.
- ^ PMID 25082925.
- ^ PMID 22643216.
- ^ PMID 29276345.
- ^ ISBN 978-1-4729-1564-1.
- ^ a b c Barkun 2003, p. 58.
- ^ ASIN B000J0N8NC.
- ^ ISBN 978-0-300-09000-0. Archivedfrom the original on 17 December 2019. Retrieved 6 August 2019.
- ^ ISBN 978-0-8166-5494-9.
- ^ PMID 30555188.
- ^ S2CID 48880069.
- ^ ISBN 978-1-4729-1564-1.
- ^ a b c d e f g h i j Lewandowsky, S.; Cook, J. (2020). The Conspiracy Theory Handbook. John Cook, Center for Climate Change Communication, George Mason University. Retrieved 17 November 2021.
- ^ Oxford English Dictionary Second Edition on CD-ROM (v. 4.0), Oxford University Press, 2009, s.v. 4
- JSTOR 1837085.
The claim that [David R.] Atchison was the originator of the [Missouri Compromise] repeal may be termed a recrudescence of the conspiracy theory first asserted by Colonel John A. Parker of Virginia in 1880.
- OCLC 4642826321.
The theory of Dr. Sankey as to the manner in which these injuries to the chest occurred in asylums deserved our careful attention. It was at least more plausible that the conspiracy theory of Mr. Charles Reade, and the precautionary measure suggested by Dr. Sankey of using a padded waistcoat in recent cases of mania with general paralysis—in which mental condition nearly all these cases under discussion were—seemed to him of practical value.
- ^ a b Bristed, C. A. (11 January 1863). "English Insincerity on the Slavery Question". The New York Times. p. 3. Retrieved 2 March 2022.
- S2CID 233242206.
- ^ Blaskiewicz, Robert (8 August 2013). "Nope, It Was Always Already Wrong". The Skeptical Inquirer. Committee for Skeptical Inquiry. Archived from the original on 12 December 2015. Retrieved 11 December 2015.
- ^ McKenzie-McHarg, Andrew (2019) "Conspiracy Theory: The Nineteenth-Century Prehistory of a Twentieth-Century Concept", pp. 78, 76. In Joseph E. Uscinski (ed) Conspiracy Theories & the People Who Believe Them. New York: Oxford University Press.
- ^ Monbiot, George (4 May 2024). "'You're going to call me a Holocaust denier now, are you?': George Monbiot comes face to face with his local conspiracy theorist". The Guardian. Retrieved 4 May 2024.
- ISBN 978-1-4729-1564-1.
- ISBN 9780292743793. Archivedfrom the original on 6 September 2016. Retrieved 27 January 2016.
The term 'conspiracy theory' did not exist as a phrase in everyday American conversation before 1964. ... In 1964, the year the Warren Commission issued its report, The New York Times published five stories in which 'conspiracy theory' appeared.
- ^ Butter, Michael (16 March 2020). "There's a conspiracy theory that the CIA invented the term 'conspiracy theory' – here's why". The Conversation. The Conversation Trust (UK) Limited. Retrieved 23 November 2020.
- ^ S2CID 141788005. Archived from the original(PDF) on 7 October 2013.
- ^ "History's greatest conspiracy theories". The Daily Telegraph. 12 November 2008. Archived from the original on 12 March 2018. Retrieved 5 April 2018.
- ISBN 978-0-230-34921-6.
- ^ Adam M. Enders, "Conspiratorial Thinking and Political Constraint". Public Opinion Quarterly 83.3 (2019): 510–533.
- ISBN 978-0-8223-3024-0. Archivedfrom the original on 22 January 2017. Retrieved 18 August 2016.
- ^ a b Kahn, Brian (2 November 2017). "There's a Damn Good Chance Your Neighbor Thinks Chemtrails Are Real". Gizmodo Earther. Archived from the original on 7 March 2019. Retrieved 5 March 2019.
- ^ Wood, M. (2015). "Has the Internet been good for conspiracy theorising?" (PDF). Psychology Postgraduate Affairs Group (PsyPAG) Quarterly (88): 31–33. Archived (PDF) from the original on 13 August 2015. Retrieved 12 September 2015.
- from the original on 24 September 2015. Retrieved 12 September 2015.
- S2CID 149762298.
- doi:10.1086/509680.
- ^ Hunt, Albert R. (3 April 2011). "Republicans Ride Theories of the Fringe". The New York Times. Bloomberg News. Archived from the original on 8 April 2011. Retrieved 23 April 2020.
- ^ "Even If It's 'Bonkers,' Poll Finds Many Believe QAnon And Other Conspiracy Theories". NPR.
- .
- S2CID 159357706.
- ^ Jesse Walker, The United States of Paranoia: A Conspiracy Theory (2013) excerpt and text search Archived 12 May 2019 at the Wayback Machine
- ^ Barkun 2003, p. 6.
- Journal of Libertarian Studies. 20 (1): 17–51. Retrieved 24 August 2023.
- ^ Barkun 2003, p. 7.
- ISBN 978-1-55164-002-0.
- ISBN 9780791473344. Archivedfrom the original on 18 April 2019. Retrieved 16 June 2015.
- ISBN 9780230349216. Archivedfrom the original on 25 January 2014. Retrieved 16 June 2015.
- ^ doi:10.1002/acp.2995.
- ^ ISBN 978-1-84520-143-2.
- ^ Birchall, Clare (2004). "Just because you're paranoid, doesn't mean they're not out to get you". Culture Machine, Deconstruction Is/In Cultural Studies. 6. Archived from the original on 23 September 2015. Retrieved 11 March 2015.
- ^ ISBN 978-1-57607-812-9. Archivedfrom the original on 6 September 2016. Retrieved 27 January 2016.
- . Retrieved 24 April 2024.
It also seems to be the case that the way we normally use the term conspiracy theory excludes instances where the theory has been generally accepted as true...Just as the Watergate scandal is now part of the official account of the Nixon administration, the NSA monitoring practices are arguably also part of our present understanding of the way that US intelligence works and neither thus qualify as 'conspiracy theories' anymore. The point here is that when we employ the term 'conspiracy theory' in actual language use, we are implicitly assuming and implying that the claims advanced by the theory are not true.
- ^ Ron Rosenbaum (2012). "Ah, Watergate". New Republic. Archived from the original on 6 August 2016. Retrieved 29 June 2016.
- ^ Bigliardi, Stefano (July–August 2020). "Who's Afraid Of Conspiracy Theory Theory?". Skeptical Inquirer. Amherst, New York: Center for Inquiry.
- ^ Novella, Steven, et al. The Skeptics' Guide to the Universe: How to Know What's Really Real in a World Increasingly Full of Fake. Grand Central Publishing, 2018. p. 208.
- ^ Nuccitelli, Dana (23 October 2013). "Fox News defends global warming false balance by denying the 97% consensus". The Guardian. Retrieved 5 October 2023.
- .
- ^ S2CID 255910928.
- ^ a b c Linden, Sander van der (30 April 2013). "Why People Believe in Conspiracy Theories". Scientific American. Retrieved 16 October 2020.
- ISBN 978-1-4729-1564-1.
- ^ Tutella, Francisco; University, Pennsylvania State. "Brief scientific literacy interventions may quash new conspiracy theories". Retrieved 2 December 2024.
- ^ Hedrih, Vladimir (11 July 2024). "Scientific literacy undermines conspiracy beliefs". PsyPost – Psychology News. Retrieved 2 December 2024.
- ^ .
- ^ PMID 34006153.
- ^ ISBN 978-0-521-80441-7.
- ^ license.
- .
- ^ Aftab, Awais (2021). "There and Back Again: Joseph Pierre, M.D." Psychiatric Times. 38 (1).
- PMID 38022962.
- ^ a b c d e Moyer, Melinda Wenner (1 March 2019). "People Drawn to Conspiracy Theories Share a Cluster of Psychological Features". Scientific American. Retrieved 16 October 2020.
- ^ PMID 32905023.)
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link - ^ PMID 33837144.
- ^ S2CID 145632390.
- ^ .
- ^ ISBN 978-0-313-24393-6.
- (PDF) from the original on 3 November 2018. Retrieved 28 December 2018.
- S2CID 43823184.
- .
- .
- S2CID 43823184.
- PMID 25762969.
- S2CID 219744361.
- S2CID 214735855.
- ISBN 978-1-317-59952-4.
- S2CID 197707663.
- ^ PMID 30853921.
- ^ a b Barkun 2003, p. 3.
- ^ a b c Berlet, Chip (September 2004). "Interview: Michael Barkun". Archived from the original on 2 April 2009. Retrieved 1 October 2009.
The issue of conspiracism versus rational criticism is a tough one, and some people (Jodi Dean, for example) argue that the former is simply a variety of the latter. I don't accept this, although I certainly acknowledge that there have been conspiracies. They simply don't have the attributes of almost superhuman power and cunning that conspiracists attribute to them.
- ^ Imhoff, Roland (17 April 2018). "Conspiracy Theorists Just Want to Feel Special". motherboard.vice.com. Archived from the original on 28 April 2019. Retrieved 6 July 2018.
- ISBN 978-0-8050-0568-4.
- ^ a b Fox, Justin (1 June 2009). "Wall Streeters like conspiracy theories. Always have". Time. Archived from the original on 2 March 2012.
- from the original on 31 August 2006. Retrieved 7 August 2006.
- S2CID 8717161.
- ^ ISBN 978-0-674-65461-7. Archivedfrom the original on 18 April 2019. Retrieved 27 October 2018.
- ^ ISBN 978-0-470-18408-0.
- ^ a b c Cohen, Roger (20 December 2010). "The Captive Arab Mind". The New York Times. Archived from the original on 25 June 2017. Retrieved 18 February 2017.
- ISBN 978-1-57230-562-5. Archived from the original on 16 December 2019. Retrieved 9 November 2019.[page needed]
- PMID 21751999.
- .
- PMID 25762969.
- .
- ^ Bolton, Doug (2 December 2015). "Scientists find a link between low intelligence and acceptance of 'pseudo-profound bulls***'". The Independent.
- PMID 29276345.
- ^ Cassam, Quassim (13 March 2015). "Bad Thinkers". Aeon.
- ^ van Prooijen, Jan-Willem, and Van Vugt, Mark (2018) "Conspiracy theories: Evolved functions and psychological mechanisms" Perspectives on Psychological Science v.13, n.6, pp.770–788
- ^ Danesi, Marcel (30 July 2023) "What Donald Trump, Vladimir Putin and Viktor Orbán Understand About Your Brain" Politico
- ^ Uscinski, Joseph E. (2 July 2019). "Conspiring for the Common Good". Skeptical Inquirer. Center for Inquiry. Archived from the original on 2 April 2020. Retrieved 9 February 2020.
- 11 September attackswere planned by the Israeli Mossad—they deflect responsibility or keep people from acknowledging that tragic events sometimes happen inexplicably."
- ISBN 978-1-78238-828-9.
- (PDF) from the original on 16 September 2012, retrieved 16 September 2012
- ^ Kelly, Michael (12 June 1995). "THE ROAD TO PARANOIA". The New Yorker. Archived from the original on 9 April 2018. Retrieved 9 April 2018.
- ^ Barkun 2003, p. 230.
- ^ Barkun 2003, pp. 207, 210, 211.
- ^ Barkun 2003, pp. 193, 197.
- ISBN 978-1538760536.
- ^ Barajas, Joshua (15 February 2016). "How many people does it take to keep a conspiracy alive?". PBS News Hour. PBS. Archived from the original on 13 October 2017. Retrieved 22 July 2016.
- PMID 26812482.
- ^ Novella, Steven, et al. The Skeptics' Guide to the Universe: How to Know What's Really Real in a World Increasingly Full of Fake. Grand Central Publishing, 2018. pp. 209–210.
- ^ "On the Viability of Conspiratorial Beliefs". PubPeer. Archived from the original on 22 March 2024.
- ^ Tiffany, Kaitlyn (17 March 2021). "The truth seekers are coming". The Atlantic. Archived from the original on 24 January 2023. Retrieved 24 January 2023.
- ^ Evans, Steve (16 May 2022). "'Rabbit hole of a conspiracy cult': 'Cooker' watching a new ACT pursuit". The Canberra Times. Archived from the original on 14 January 2023. Retrieved 24 January 2023.
- ^ Withers, Rachel (17 November 2022). "Cooking up a storm". The Monthly. Retrieved 24 January 2023.
- ^ "'Cookers' are a product of the modern Left". The Spectator Australia. 16 December 2022. Retrieved 24 January 2023.
- ^ "International Poll: No Consensus on Who Was Behind 9/11". WorldPublicOpinion.org. University of Maryland, College Park: Program on International Policy Attitudes. 10 September 2008. Archived from the original on 5 July 2011.
- ^ Popper, Karl (1945). "14". Open Society and Its Enemies, Book II. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
- ^ "Extracts from "The Open Society and Its Enemies Volume 2: The High Tide of Prophecy: Hegel, Marx and the Aftermath" by Karl Raimund Popper (Originally published 1945)". Lachlan Cranswick, quoting Karl Raimund Popper. Archived from the original on 3 September 2006. Retrieved 5 September 2006.
- ^ a b Cumings, Bruce (1999). The Origins of the Korean War, Vol. II, The Roaring of the Cataract, 1947–1950. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.[page needed]
- . Retrieved 14 July 2021.
- ^ ISBN 978-0-415-57518-8.
- ^ Wakin, Daniel J. (26 October 2002). "Anti-Semitic 'Elders of Zion' Gets New Life on Egypt TV". The New York Times. Archived from the original on 16 August 2014. Retrieved 26 August 2014.
- ^ "2006 Saudi Arabia's Curriculum of Intolerance" (PDF). Archived from the original (PDF) on 23 August 2006. Report by Center for Religious Freedom of Freedom House. 2006
- ^ "The Booksellers of Tehran" Archived 10 April 2017 at the Wayback Machine, The Wall Street Journal, 28 October 2005
- ^ Steven Stalinsky (6 May 2004). "A Vast Conspiracy". National Review. Archived from the original on 4 October 2013.
- ISBN 978-1-136-96751-1.
- ^ a b Mustafa Akyol (12 September 2016). "The Tin-Foil Hats Are Out in Turkey". Foreign Policy. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017. Retrieved 10 January 2017.
- ^ Selim Koru (21 June 2018). "How Nietzsche Explains Turkey". The Atlantic. Archived from the original on 21 June 2018. Retrieved 21 June 2018.
- ^ Marin Lessenski (March 2018). "COMMON SENSE WANTED – Resilence to 'post-truth' and its predictors in the new media literacy index 2018" (PDF). Open Society Institute – Sofia. Archived from the original (PDF) on 3 April 2018. Retrieved 6 April 2018.
- S2CID 159483845– via Project MUSE.
- .
- ^ "In Turkey, conspiracy theories about the Peace Treaty of Lausanne run riot". The Skeptic. 29 March 2023. Archived from the original on 2 July 2023. Retrieved 17 May 2024.
- ^ "Lozan Antlaşması'nın 100. Yılında Komplo Teorileri ve Gizli Maddelerin İzinde". Yalansavar (in Turkish). 25 July 2023. Archived from the original on 6 August 2023. Retrieved 17 May 2024.
- ISBN 9781848856110.
- ISBN 978-0-674-44302-0. ASIN: B000NUF6FQ.[page needed]
- ^ Harry G. West; et al. Transparency and Conspiracy: Ethnographies of Suspicion in the New World Order. Duke University Press Books. pp. 4, 207–08.
- ^ Shermer, Michael, and Pat Linse. Conspiracy Theories. Altadena, CA: Skeptics Society, n.d. Print.
- ^ Jewett, Robert; John Shelton Lawrence (2004) Captain America and the Crusade Against Evil: The Dilemma of Zealous Nationalism Archived 18 April 2019 at the Wayback Machine Wm. B. Eerdmans Publishing p. 206.
- ISBN 978-0-520-06469-0.
- ^ Olmsted, Kathryn S. (2011). Real Enemies: Conspiracy Theories and American Democracy, World War I to 9/11 Archived 18 April 2019 at the Wayback Machine. Oxford University Press, p. 8.
- ^ Friedersdorf, Conor (29 October 2011). "Ron Paul, Conspiracy Theories, and the Right". The Atlantic. Retrieved 30 August 2020.
- ^ Stack, Liam (3 October 2016). "He Calls Hillary Clinton a 'Demon.' Who Is Alex Jones?" The New York Times
- S2CID 231654586.
- S2CID 222094116.
- ^ Roose, Kevin (3 September 2021) [4 March 2021]. "What Is QAnon, the Viral Pro-Trump Conspiracy Theory?". The New York Times. New York City. Archived from the original on 19 September 2021. Retrieved 25 September 2021.
- ^ Bowman, Emma (4 February 2021). "Why QAnon Survives After Trump". Washington, D.C.: NPR. Archived from the original on 5 September 2021. Retrieved 25 September 2021.
Further reading
- ISBN 978-1-59448-895-5.
- Arnold, Gordon B., ed. (2008). Conspiracy Theory in Film, Television, and Politics. ISBN 978-0-275-99462-4.
- Burnett, Thom. Conspiracy Encyclopedia: The Encyclopedia of Conspiracy Theories
- Butter, Michael; Peter Knight (November 2015). "Bridging the Great Divide: Conspiracy Theory Research for the 21st Century". Diogenes. 62 (3–4): 17–29. S2CID 152067265.
- Chase, Alston (2003). Harvard and the Unabomber: The Education of an American Terrorist. New York: W. W. Norton. ISBN 978-0-393-02002-1.
- Coward, Barry, ed. (2004). Conspiracies and Conspiracy Theory in Early Modern Europe: From the Waldensians to the French Revolution. ISBN 978-0-7546-3564-2.
- "Conspiracy Theories" (PDF). CQ Researcher. 19 (37): 885–908. 23 October 2009. Archived (PDF) from the original on 9 October 2022.
- Cziesche, Dominik; Jürgen Dahlkamp; Ulrich Fichtner; Ulrich Jaeger; Gunther Latsch; Gisela Leske; Max F. Ruppert (2003). "Panoply of the Absurd". Der Spiegel. Retrieved 28 November 2023.
- De Graaf, Beatrice and Zwierlein, Cornel (eds.) "Security and Conspiracy in History, 16th to 21st Century". Historical Social Research 38, Special Issue, 2013
- Fleming, Chris and Emma A. Jane. Modern Conspiracy: The Importance of Being Paranoid. New York and London: Bloomsbury, 2014. ISBN 978-1-62356-091-1.
- Goertzel, Ted. "Belief in conspiracy theories". Political Psychology (1994): 731–742. online Archived 31 August 2006 at the Wayback Machine
- OCLC 1407420009.
- Harris, Lee. "The Trouble with Conspiracy Theories". The American, 12 January 2013.
- Hofstadter, Richard. The Paranoid Style in American Politics (1954). online
- Johnson, George (1983). Architects of Fear: Conspiracy Theories and Paranoia in American Politics. Los Angeles: Jeremy P. Tarcher. ISBN 978-0-87477-275-3.
- McConnachie, James; Tudge, Robin (2005). The Rough Guide to Conspiracy Theories. Rough Guides. ISBN 978-1-84353-445-7.
- Melley, Timothy (1999). Empire of Conspiracy: The Culture of Paranoia in Postwar America. Ithaca, New York: Cornell University Press. ISBN 978-0-8014-8606-7.
- Meigs, James B. (2006). "The Conspiracy Industry". Popular Mechanics. Archived from the original on 24 October 2006. Retrieved 13 October 2006.
- Nefes, Türkay Salim (2012). "The history of the social constructions of Dönmes". Journal of Historical Sociology. 25 (3): 413–39. .
- Nefes, Türkay Salim (2013). "Political parties' perceptions and uses of anti-Semitic conspiracy theories in Turkey". The Sociological Review. 61 (2): 247–64. S2CID 145632390.
- Oliver, J. Eric, and Thomas J. Wood. "Conspiracy theories and the paranoid style(s) of mass opinion". American Journal of Political Science 58.4 (2014): 952–966.online
- Parsons, Charlotte (24 September 2001). "Why we need conspiracy theories". Americas. BBC News. Retrieved 26 June 2006.
- Pipes, Daniel (1998). ISBN 978-0-312-17688-4.
- Pipes, Daniel (1997). ISBN 978-0-684-87111-0.
- Pigden, Charles (1995). "Popper Revisited, or What Is Wrong with Conspiracy Theories?". Philosophy of the Social Sciences. 25 (1): 3–34. S2CID 143602969.
- ISBN 978-0-394-53512-8.
- Slosson, W. "The 'Conspiracy' Superstition". The Unpopular Review, Vol. VII, No. 14, 1917.
- Sunstein, Cass R., and Adrian Vermeule. "Conspiracy theories: Causes and cures". Journal of Political Philosophy 17.2 (2009): 202–227. online
- Uscinski, Joseph E. and Joseph M. Parent, American Conspiracy Theories (2014) excerpt
- Uscinski, Joseph E. "The 5 Most Dangerous Conspiracy Theories of 2016". Politico Magazine (22 Aug 2016)
- ISBN 978-0-8065-2531-0.
- Wood, Gordon S. (1982). "Conspiracy and the Paranoid Style: Causality and Deceit in the Eighteenth Century". The William and Mary Quarterly. 39 (3): 402–441. JSTOR 1919580.