Time perception

Source: Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia.
(Redirected from
Perception of time
)

The study of time perception or chronoception is a field within psychology, cognitive linguistics[1] and neuroscience that refers to the subjective experience, or sense, of time, which is measured by someone's own perception of the duration of the indefinite and unfolding of events.[2][3][4] The perceived time interval between two successive events is referred to as perceived duration. Though directly experiencing or understanding another person's perception of time is not possible, perception can be objectively studied and inferred through a number of scientific experiments. Some temporal illusions help to expose the underlying neural mechanisms of time perception.

The ancient Greeks recognized the difference between chronological time (chronos) and subjective time (kairos).

Pioneering work on time perception, emphasizing species-specific differences, was conducted by Karl Ernst von Baer.[5]

Theories

Time perception is typically categorized in three distinct ranges, because different ranges of duration are processed in different areas of the brain:[6]

  • Sub-second timing or millisecond timing
  • Interval timing or seconds-to-minutes timing
  • Circadian timing

There are many theories and computational models for time perception mechanisms in the brain. William J. Friedman (1993) contrasted two theories of the sense of time:[7][8][9]

  • The strength model of time memory. This posits a memory trace that persists over time, by which one might judge the age of a memory (and therefore how long ago the event remembered occurred) from the strength of the trace. This conflicts with the fact that memories of recent events may fade more quickly than more distant memories.
  • The inference model suggests the time of an event is inferred from information about relations between the event in question and other events whose date or time is known.

Another hypothesis involves the brain's subconscious tallying of "pulses" during a specific interval, forming a biological stopwatch. This theory proposes that the brain can run multiple biological stopwatches independently depending on the type of tasks being tracked. The source and nature of the pulses is unclear.[10] They are as yet a metaphor whose correspondence to brain anatomy or physiology is unknown.[11]

Philosophical perspectives

The

Merleau-Ponty.[15]

Although the perception of time is not associated with a specific sensory system,

ultradian) timekeeping. There is some evidence that very short (millisecond) durations are processed by dedicated neurons in early sensory parts of the brain.[18][19]

parietal cortex, and left premotor cortex. Explicit timing often involves the supplementary motor area and the right prefrontal cortex.[11]

Two visual stimuli, inside someone's field of view, can be successfully regarded as simultaneous up to five milliseconds.[20][21][22]

In the popular essay "Brain Time", David Eagleman explains that different types of sensory information (auditory, tactile, visual, etc.) are processed at different speeds by different neural architectures. The brain must learn how to overcome these speed disparities if it is to create a temporally unified representation of the external world:

if the visual brain wants to get events correct timewise, it may have only one choice: wait for the slowest information to arrive. To accomplish this, it must wait about a tenth of a second. In the early days of television broadcasting, engineers worried about the problem of keeping audio and video signals synchronized. Then they accidentally discovered that they had around a hundred milliseconds of slop: As long as the signals arrived within this window, viewers' brains would automatically resynchronize the signals. He goes on to say, "This brief waiting period allows the visual system to discount the various delays imposed by the early stages; however, it has the disadvantage of pushing perception into the past. There is a distinct survival advantage to operating as close to the present as possible; an animal does not want to live too far in the past. Therefore, the tenth-of-a-second window may be the smallest delay that allows higher areas of the brain to account for the delays created in the first stages of the system while still operating near the border of the present. This window of delay means that awareness is retroactive, incorporating data from a window of time after an event and delivering a delayed interpretation of what happened."[23]

Experiments have shown that rats can successfully estimate a time interval of approximately 40 seconds, despite having their cortex entirely removed.[24] This suggests that time estimation may be a low-level process.[25]

Ecological perspectives

In recent history,

humans.[26]

There is

flies), which have a fast metabolic rate, experience time more slowly than animals of larger size, which have a slow metabolic rate.[28][29] Researchers suppose that this could be the reason why small-bodied animals are generally better at perceiving time on a small scale, and why they are more agile than larger animals.[30]

Time perception in vertebrates

Examples in fish

In a lab experiment,

electric shock, with a constant time interval between the two stimuli. Test subjects showed an increase in general activity around the time of the electric shock. This response persisted in further trials in which the light stimulus was kept but the electric shock was removed.[31]
This suggests that goldfish are able to perceive time intervals and to initiate an avoidance response at the time when they expect the distressing stimulus to happen.

In two separate studies, golden shiners and dwarf inangas demonstrated the ability to associate the availability of food sources to specific locations and times of day, called time-place learning.[32][33] In contrast, when tested for time-place learning based on predation risk, inangas were unable to associate spatiotemporal patterns to the presence or absence of predators.

In June 2022, researchers reported in

counter-intuitive responses to the arrow of time in how their eyes perceived different stimuli.[34]

Examples in birds

When presented with the choice between obtaining food at regular intervals (with a fixed delay between feedings) or at stochastic intervals (with a variable delay between feedings), starlings can discriminate between the two types of intervals and consistently prefer getting food at variable intervals. This is true whether the total amount of food is the same for both options or if the total amount of food is unpredictable in the variable option. This suggests that starlings have an inclination for risk-prone behavior.[35]

visual cues.[39]

Examples in mammals

A study on privately owned dogs revealed that

dogs are able to perceive durations ranging from minutes to several hours differently. Dogs reacted with increasing intensity to the return of their owners when they were left alone for longer durations, regardless of the owners' behavior.[40]

After being trained with food

wild boars are able to correctly estimate time intervals of days by asking for food at the end of each interval, but they are unable to accurately estimate time intervals of minutes with the same training method.[41]

When trained with

positive reinforcement, rats can learn to respond to a signal of a certain duration, but not to signals of shorter or longer durations, which demonstrates that they can discriminate between different durations.[42] Rats have demonstrated time-place learning, and can also learn to infer correct timing for a specific task by following an order of events, suggesting that they might be able to use an ordinal timing mechanism.[43] Like pigeons, rats are thought to have the ability to use a circadian timing mechanism for discriminating time of day.[44]

Time perception in invertebrates

Forager honey bee flying back to the hive with pollen and nectar

When returning to the

general anesthesia significantly delayed the start of the foraging behaviour of honeybees if induced during daytime, but not if induced during nighttime.[48]

Bumble bees can be successfully trained to respond to a stimulus after a certain time interval has elapsed (usually several seconds after the start signal). Studies have shown that they can also learn to simultaneously time multiple interval durations.[49]

In a single study, colonies from three species of

ants from the genus Myrmica were trained to associate feeding sessions with different times. The trainings lasted several days, where each day the feeding time was delayed by 20 minutes compared to the previous day. In all three species, at the end of the training, most individuals were present at the feeding spot at the correct expected times, suggesting that ants are able to estimate the time running, keep in memory the expected feeding time and to act anticipatively.[50]

Types of temporal illusions

A temporal illusion is a distortion in the perception of time. For example:

  • estimating time intervals, e.g., "When did you last see your primary care physician?";
  • estimating time duration, e.g., "How long were you waiting at the doctor's office?"; and
  • judging the simultaneity of events (see below for examples).
Main types of temporal illusions
  • Telescoping effect: People tend to recall recent events as occurring further back in time than they actually did (backward telescoping) and distant events as occurring more recently than they actually did (forward telescoping).[51]
  • Vierordt's law: Shorter intervals tend to be overestimated while longer intervals tend to be underestimated
  • Time intervals associated with more changes may be perceived as longer than intervals with fewer changes
  • Perceived temporal length of a given task may shorten with greater motivation
  • Perceived temporal length of a given task may stretch when broken up or interrupted
  • Auditory stimuli may appear to last longer than visual stimuli[52][53][54][55]
  • Time durations may appear longer with greater stimulus intensity (e.g., auditory loudness or pitch)
  • Simultaneity judgments can be manipulated by repeated exposure to non-simultaneous stimuli

Kappa effect

The Kappa effect or perceptual time dilation[56] is a form of temporal illusion verifiable by experiment.[57] The temporal duration between a sequence of consecutive stimuli is thought to be relatively longer or shorter than its actual elapsed time, due to the spatial/auditory/tactile separation between each consecutive stimuli. The kappa effect can be displayed when considering a journey made in two parts that each take an equal amount of time. When mentally comparing these two sub-journeys, the part that covers more distance may appear to take longer than the part covering less distance, even though they take an equal amount of time.

Eye movements and chronostasis

The perception of space and time undergoes distortions during rapid saccadic eye movements.[58]

eye movement). This elicits an overestimation in the temporal duration for which that target stimulus (i.e., postsaccadic stimulus) was perceived. This effect can extend apparent durations by up to 500 ms and is consistent with the idea that the visual system models events prior to perception.[60] The most well-known version of this illusion is known as the stopped-clock illusion, wherein a subject's first impression of the second-hand movement of an analog clock, subsequent to one's directed attention (i.e., saccade) to the clock, is the perception of a slower-than-normal second-hand movement rate (the seconds hand of the clock may seemingly temporarily freeze in place after initially looking at it).[61][62][63][64]

The occurrence of chronostasis extends beyond the visual domain into the

tactile domains.[65] In the auditory domain, chronostasis and duration overestimation occur when observing auditory stimuli. One common example is a frequent occurrence when making telephone calls. If, while listening to the phone's dial tone, research subjects move the phone from one ear to the other, the length of time between rings appears longer.[66] In the tactile domain, chronostasis has persisted in research subjects as they reach for and grasp objects. After grasping a new object, subjects overestimate the time in which their hand has been in contact with this object.[62]

Flash-lag effect

In an experiment, participants were told to stare at an "x" symbol on a computer screen whereby a moving blue doughnut-like ring repeatedly circled the fixed "x" point.[67][68][69] Occasionally, the ring would display a white flash for a split second that physically overlapped the ring's interior. However, when asked what was perceived, participants responded that they saw the white flash lagging behind the center of the moving ring. In other words, despite the reality that the two retinal images were actually spatially aligned, the flashed object was usually observed to trail a continuously moving object in space — a phenomenon referred to as the flash-lag effect.

The first proposed explanation, called the "motion extrapolation" hypothesis, is that the visual system extrapolates the position of moving objects but not flashing objects when accounting for neural delays (i.e., the lag time between the retinal image and the observer's perception of the flashing object). The second proposed explanation by David Eagleman and Sejnowski, called the "latency difference" hypothesis, is that the visual system processes moving objects at a faster rate than flashed objects. In the attempt to disprove the first hypothesis, David Eagleman conducted an experiment in which the moving ring suddenly reverses direction to spin in the other way as the flashed object briefly appears. If the first hypothesis were correct, we would expect that, immediately following reversal, the moving object would be observed as lagging behind the flashed object. However, the experiment revealed the opposite — immediately following reversal, the flashed object was observed as lagging behind the moving object. This experimental result supports the "latency difference" hypothesis. A recent study tries to reconcile these different approaches by treating perception as an inference mechanism aiming at describing what is happening at the present time.[70]

Oddball effect

Humans typically overestimate the perceived duration of the initial and final event in a stream of identical events.[71] This oddball effect may serve an evolutionarily adapted "alerting" function and is consistent with reports of time slowing down in threatening situations. The effect seems to be strongest for images that are expanding in size on the retina, in other words, that are "looming" or approaching the viewer,[72][73][74] and the effect can be eradicated for oddballs that are contracting or perceived to be receding from the viewer.[73] The effect is also reduced[72] or reversed[74] with a static oddball presented among a stream of expanding stimuli.

Initial studies suggested that this oddball-induced "subjective time dilation" expanded the perceived duration of oddball stimuli by 30–50%[72] but subsequent research has reported more modest expansion of around 10%[74][75][76][77] or less.[78] The direction of the effect, whether the viewer perceives an increase or a decrease in duration, also seems to be dependent upon the stimulus used.[78]

Reversal of temporal order judgment

Numerous experimental findings suggest that temporal order judgments of actions preceding effects can be reversed under special circumstances. Experiments have shown that sensory simultaneity judgments can be manipulated by repeated exposure to non-simultaneous stimuli. In an experiment conducted by David Eagleman, a temporal order judgment reversal was induced in subjects by exposing them to delayed motor consequences. In the experiment, subjects played various forms of video games. Unknown to the subjects, the experimenters introduced a fixed delay between the mouse movements and the subsequent sensory feedback. For example, a subject may not see a movement register on the screen until 150 milliseconds after they had moved the mouse. Participants playing the game quickly adapted to the delay and felt as though there was less delay between their mouse movement and the sensory feedback. Shortly after the experimenters removed the delay, the subjects commonly felt as though the effect on the screen happened just before they commanded it. This work addresses how the perceived timing of effects is modulated by expectations, and the extent to which such predictions are quickly modifiable.[79]

In an experiment conducted by Haggard and colleagues in 2002, participants pressed a button that triggered a flash of light at a distance, after a slight delay of 100 milliseconds.[80] By repeatedly engaging in this act, participants had adapted to the delay (i.e., they experienced a gradual shortening in the perceived time interval between pressing the button and seeing the flash of light). The experimenters then showed the flash of light instantly after the button was pressed. In response, subjects often thought that the flash (the effect) had occurred before the button was pressed (the cause). Additionally, when the experimenters slightly reduced the delay, and shortened the spatial distance between the button and the flash of light, participants had often claimed again to have experienced the effect before the cause.

Several experiments also suggest that temporal order judgment of a pair of

tactile stimuli delivered in rapid succession, one to each hand, is noticeably impaired (i.e., misreported) by crossing the hands over the midline. However, congenitally blind subjects showed no trace of temporal order judgment reversal after crossing the arms. These results suggest that tactile signals taken in by the congenitally blind are ordered in time without being referred to a visuospatial representation. Unlike the congenitally blind subjects, the temporal order judgments of the late-onset blind subjects were impaired when crossing the arms to a similar extent as non-blind subjects. These results suggest that the associations between tactile signals and visuospatial representation is maintained once it is accomplished during infancy. Some research studies have also found that the subjects showed reduced deficit in tactile temporal order judgments when the arms were crossed behind their back than when they were crossed in front.[81][82][83]

Physiological associations

Tachypsychia

Tachypsychia is a neurological condition that alters the perception of time, usually induced by

drug use, or a traumatic event. For someone affected by tachypsychia, time perceived by the individual either lengthens, making events appear to slow down,[84] or contracts, with objects appearing as moving in a speeding blur.[85][86]

Effects of emotional states

Awe

Research has suggested the feeling of awe has the ability to expand one's perceptions of time availability. Awe can be characterized as an experience of immense perceptual vastness that coincides with an increase in focus. Consequently, it is conceivable that one's temporal perception would slow down when experiencing awe.[87] The perception of time can differ as people choose between savoring moments and deferring gratification.[88]

Fear

Possibly related to the

skydives or bungee jumps, where they are capable of complex thoughts in what would normally be the blink of an eye (See Fight-or-flight response).[89] This reported slowing in temporal perception may have been evolutionarily advantageous because it may have enhanced one's ability to intelligibly make quick decisions in moments that were of critical importance to our survival.[90] However, even though observers commonly report that time seems to have moved in slow motion during these events, it is unclear whether this is a function of increased time resolution during the event, or instead an illusion created by the remembering of an emotionally salient event.[91]

A strong time dilation effect has been reported for perception of objects that were looming, but not of those retreating, from the viewer, suggesting that the expanding discs — which mimic an approaching object — elicit

epinephrine, which increases brain activity (an adrenaline rush).[93] In such circumstances, an illusion of time dilation could assist an effective escape.[94][95] When exposed to a threat, three-year-old children were observed to exhibit a similar tendency to overestimate elapsed time.[11][96]

Research suggests that the effect appears only at the point of retrospective assessment, rather than occurring simultaneously with events as they happened.[97] Perceptual abilities were tested during a frightening experience — a free fall — by measuring people's sensitivity to flickering stimuli. The results showed that the subjects' temporal resolution was not improved as the frightening event was occurring. Events appear to have taken longer only in retrospect, possibly because memories were being more densely packed during the frightening situation.[97]

Other researchers[98][99] suggest that additional variables could lead to a different state of consciousness in which altered time perception does occur during an event. Research does demonstrate that visual sensory processing[100] increases in scenarios involving action preparation. Participants demonstrated a higher detection rate of rapidly presented symbols when preparing to move, as compared to a control without movement.

People shown extracts from films known to induce fear often overestimated the elapsed time of a subsequently presented visual stimulus, whereas people shown emotionally neutral clips (weather forecasts and stock market updates) or those known to evoke feelings of sadness showed no difference. It is argued that fear prompts a state of arousal in the amygdala, which increases the rate of a hypothesized "internal clock". This could be the result of an evolved defensive mechanism triggered by a threatening situation.[101] Individuals experiencing sudden or surprising events, real or imagined (e.g., witnessing a crime, or believing one is seeing a ghost), may overestimate the duration of the event.[88]

Changes with age

Psychologists have found that the subjective perception of the passing of time tends to speed up with increasing age in humans. This often causes people to increasingly underestimate a given interval of time as they age. This fact can likely be attributed to a variety of age-related changes in the aging brain, such as the lowering in dopaminergic levels with older age; however, the details are still being debated.[102][103][104]

Very young children will first experience the passing of time when they can subjectively perceive and reflect on the unfolding of a collection of events. A child's awareness of time develops during childhood, when the child's attention and short-term memory capacities form — this developmental process is thought to be dependent on the slow maturation of the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus.[11][105]

The common explanation is that most external and internal experiences are new for young children but repetitive for adults. Children have to be extremely engaged (i.e. dedicate many neural resources or significant brain power) in the present moment because they must constantly reconfigure their mental models of the world to assimilate it and manage behaviour properly.

Adults, however, may rarely need to step outside mental habits and external routines. When an adult frequently experiences the same stimuli, such stimuli may seem "invisible" as a result of having already been sufficiently mapped by the brain. This phenomenon is known as neural adaptation. Thus, the brain will record fewer densely rich memories during these frequent periods of disengagement from the present moment.[clarify][106] Consequently, the subjective perception is often that time passes by at a faster rate with age.

Proportional to the real time

Let S be subjective time, R be real time, and define both to be zero at birth.

One model proposes that the passage of subjective time relative to actual time is inversely proportional to real time:[107]

When solved, .

One day would be approximately 1/4,000 of the life of an 11-year-old, but approximately 1/20,000 of the life of a 55-year-old. This helps to explain why a random, ordinary day may therefore appear longer for a young child than an adult. So a year would be experienced by a 55-year-old as passing approximately five times more quickly than a year experienced by an 11-year-old. If long-term time perception is based solely on the proportionality of a person's age, then the following four periods in life would appear to be quantitatively equal: ages 5–10 (1x), ages 10–20 (2x), ages 20–40 (4x), age 40–80 (8x), as the end age is twice the start age. However, this does not work for ages 0–10, which corresponds to ages 10–∞.[107][108]

Proportional to the subjective time

Lemlich posits that the passage of subjective time relative to actual time is inversely proportional to total subjective time, rather than the total real time:[107]

When mathematically solved,

It avoids the issue of infinite subjective time passing from real age 0 to 1 year, as the asymptote can be integrated in an improper integral. Using the initial conditions S = 0 when R = 0 and K > 0,

This means that time appears to pass in proportion to the square root of the perceiver's real age, rather than directly proportional. Under this model, a 55-year-old would subjectively experience time passing 2+1/4 times more quickly than an 11-year-old, rather than five times under the previous. This means the following periods in life would appear to be quantitatively equal: ages 0–1, 1–4, 4–9, 9–16, 16–25, 25–36, 36–49, 49–64, 64–81, 81–100, 100–121.[107][109]

In a study, participants consistently provided answers that fit this model when asked about time perception at 1/4 of their age, but were less consistent for 1/2 of their age. Their answers suggest that this model is more accurate than the previous one.[107]

A consequence of this model is that the fraction of subjective life remaining is always less than the fraction of real life remaining, but it is always more than one half of real life remaining.[107] This can be seen for and :

Effects of drugs on time perception

neurotransmitters such as dopamine and norepinephrine may be the reason for this.[111][112][113] A research on stimulant-dependent individuals (SDI) showed several abnormal time processing characteristics including larger time differences for effective duration discrimination, and overestimating the duration of a relatively long time interval. Altered time processing and perception in SDI could explain the difficulty SDI have with delaying gratification.[114] Another research studied the dose-dependent effect in methamphetamine dependents with short term abstinence and its effects on time perception. Results shows that motor timing but not perceptual timing, was altered in meth dependents, which persisted for at least three months of abstinence. Dose-dependent effects on time perception were only observed when short-term abstinent meth abusers processed long time intervals. The study concluded that time perception alteration in meth dependents is task specific and dose dependent.[115]

The effect of cannabis on time perception has been studied with inconclusive results mainly due to methodological variations and the paucity of research. Even though 70% of time estimation studies report over-estimation, the findings of time production and time reproduction studies remain inconclusive.[116][117] Studies show consistently throughout the literature that most cannabis users self-report the experience of a slowed perception of time. In the laboratory, researchers have confirmed the effect of cannabis on the perception of time in both humans and animals.[118] Using PET Scans it was observed that participants who showed a decrease in cerebellar blood flow (CBF) also had a significant alteration in time sense. The relationship between decreased CBF and impaired time sense is of interest as the cerebellum is linked to an internal timing system.[119][120]

Effects of body temperature

The chemical clock hypothesis implies a causal link between body temperature and the perception of time.[121]

Past work show that increasing body temperature tends to make individuals experience a dilated perception of time and they perceive durations as shorter than they actually were, ultimately leading them to under-estimate time durations. While decreasing body temperature has the opposite effect – causing participants to experience a condensed perception of time leading them to over-estimate time duration – observations of the latter type were rare.[122] Research establishes a parametric effect of body temperature on time perception with higher temperatures generally producing faster subjective time and vice versa. This is especially seen to be true under changes in arousal levels and stressful events.[123]

Applications

Since subjective time is measurable, through information such as heartbeats or actions taken within a time period, there are analytical applications for time perception.

Social networks

Time perception can be used as a tool in

literature, analyzed by social networks. Each character's subjective time may be calculated, with methods as simple as word counting, and compared to the real time of the story to shed light on their internal states.[124][125]

See also

References

  1. .
  2. ^ Livni E (8 January 2019). "Physics explains why time passes faster as you age". Quartz. Retrieved 21 March 2019.
  3. EurekAlert!
    . Retrieved 21 March 2019.
  4. PMID 36300073
    .
  5. ^ von Baer KE (1862). Welche Auffassung der lebenden Natur ist die richtige? [Which view of living nature is the right one?] (in German). Berlin: A. Hirschwald.
  6. PMID 22065383
    .
  7. ^ Le Poidevin R (August 28, 2000). "The Experience and Perception of Time". Retrieved 2009-10-22.
  8. .
  9. .
  10. Smithsonian Magazine
    . Retrieved May 1, 2014.
  11. ^ a b c d Gozlan M (2 Jan 2013). "A stopwatch on the brain's perception of time". theguardian.com. Guardian News and Media Limited. Archived from the original on 4 January 2014. Retrieved 4 January 2014.
  12. .
  13. ^ Anonymous (E. Robert Kelly, 1882) The Alternative: A Study in Psychology. London: Macmillan and Co. p. 168.
  14. ^
    S2CID 16379171. Archived from the original
    (PDF) on 2008-02-16. Retrieved 2008-02-02.
  15. ^ Varela FJ (1999). Petitot J, Varela FJ, Pachoud B, Roy JM (eds.). "The specious present: A neurophenomenology of time consciousness". Naturalizing Phenomenology: Issues in Contemporary Phenomenology and Cognitive Science. Stanford University Press. 64: 266–329.
  16. S2CID 3570715
    .
  17. S2CID 3570715
    .
  18. .
  19. .
  20. ^ Eagleman DM (23 June 2009). "Brain Time". Edge. Edge Foundation. Archived from the original on 21 December 2013.
  21. .
  22. .
  23. ^ Eagleman DM (2009-06-23). "Brain Time". Edge Foundation. Archived from the original on 2013-08-05.
  24. S2CID 19254667
    .
  25. .
  26. .
  27. ^ Alger SJ (30 December 2013). "Metabolism and Body Size Influence the Perception of Movement and Time | Accumulating Glitches | Learn Science at Scitable". Nature. Retrieved 30 January 2020.
  28. ^ "Time passes more slowly for flies, study finds". The Guardian. Press Association. 16 September 2013.
  29. PMID 24109147
    .
  30. ^ "Time perception varies between animals". The University of Edinburgh. 2016.
  31. PMID 15656725
    .
  32. .
  33. .
  34. .
  35. .
  36. .
  37. .
  38. .
  39. .
  40. .
  41. .
  42. .
  43. .
  44. .
  45. .
  46. .
  47. .
  48. .
  49. .
  50. .
  51. ^ "It Seems Like Only Yesterday: The Nature and Consequences of Telescoping Errors in Marketing Research". Journal of Consumer Psychology.
  52. S2CID 16487453
    .
  53. .
  54. .
  55. PMID 9621837. Archived (PDF) from the original on 2013-04-21.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: DOI inactive as of January 2024 (link
    )
  56. .
  57. ^ Wada Y, Masuda T, Noguchi K, 2005, "Temporal illusion called 'kappa effect' in event perception" Perception 34 ECVP Abstract Supplement
  58. .
  59. .
  60. .
  61. .
  62. ^ .
  63. .
  64. ^ "The mystery of the stopped clock illusion". BBC - Future - Health -. 2012-08-27. Archived from the original on 2013-01-20. Retrieved 2012-12-09.
  65. .
  66. .
  67. ^ Kotler S (12 April 2010). "When Life Flashes Before Your Eyes: A 15-Story Drop to Study the Brain's Internal Timewarp". Popular Science. Bonnier Corporation. Archived from the original on 11 October 2014.
  68. ^ Eagleman DM, Sejnowski TJ (2007). "Flash-Lag Effect". Eagleman Laboratory for Perception and Action. Archived from the original on 2014-08-01.
  69. PMID 11184992. Archived from the original
    (PDF) on 2014-08-08.
  70. .
  71. .
  72. ^ .
  73. ^ .
  74. ^ .
  75. .
  76. .
  77. .
  78. ^ .
  79. S2CID 8179689. Archived from the original
    (PDF) on 2013-09-28.
  80. .
  81. (PDF) from the original on 2015-04-02.
  82. (PDF) from the original on 2013-09-28.
  83. .
  84. ^ "Ready, steady, slow! Why top sportsmen might have 'more time' on the ball". ucl.ac.uk. University College London. 6 September 2012.
  85. Nautilus (science magazine). Archived from the original
    on 7 June 2018. Retrieved 7 June 2018.
  86. .
  87. ]
  88. ^ a b Radford, Benjamin; Frazier, Kendrick (January 2017). "Felt Time: The Psychology of How We Perceive Time". Skeptical Inquirer. 41 (1): 60–61.
  89. ^ "David dives in". justRegional publishing. 13 Jul 2013. Archived from the original on 26 August 2016. Retrieved 13 July 2013.
  90. ^ Geoghagen T (2007-08-02). "Turn back the clock". BBC News Magazine.
  91. ^ Why top sport stars might have 'more time' on the ball by Jonathan Amos Science correspondent, BBC News
  92. PMID 19487187
    .
  93. .
  94. .
  95. ^ Choi CQ (11 December 2007). "Why Time Seems to Slow Down in Emergencies". Live Science.
  96. S2CID 15412640. Archived from the original
    (PDF) on 2014-01-04.
  97. ^ .
  98. .
  99. ^ Taylor, Steve (6 September 2019). "Why accidents and emergencies seem to dramatically slow down time". theconversation.com. The Conversation US, Inc.
  100. PMID 22951740
    .
  101. .
  102. .
  103. .
  104. PMID 8806025. Archived from the original
    (PDF) on 2013-10-29.
  105. .
  106. ^ Cooper BB (2013-07-02). "The science of time perception: stop it slipping away by doing new things". The Buffer Blog. Archived from the original on 2013-08-16.
  107. ^
    S2CID 20017140
    . Retrieved 2020-12-24.
  108. ^ Adler R (1999-12-25). "Look how time flies . . ". New Scientist. Archived from the original on 2011-06-14. Retrieved 2009-10-22.
  109. ^ Jo DiLonardo M (1994-02-06). "Time Does Fly As We Grow Older". Chicago Tribune. Archived from the original on 2016-04-25.
  110. ^ "Time perception - Personality traits". Encyclopedia Britannica. Retrieved 2020-06-06.
  111. ^ Gozlan M (2 Jan 2013). "A stopwatch on the brain's perception of time". theguardian.com. Guardian News and Media Limited. Archived from the original on 4 January 2014. Retrieved 4 January 2014.
  112. S2CID 8176967
    .
  113. .
  114. .
  115. .
  116. .
  117. . Retrieved 2020-06-28.
  118. ^ Stolick, Matt (2008). Otherwise Law-Abiding Citizens: A Scientific and Moral Assessment of Cannabis Use. Lexington Books. pp. 39–41.
  119. S2CID 40578680
    .
  120. .
  121. PMID 7597195.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: DOI inactive as of January 2024 (link
    )
  122. PMID 7597195.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: DOI inactive as of January 2024 (link
    )
  123. PMID 7597195.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: DOI inactive as of January 2024 (link
    )
  124. ^ Lotker, Z. (2016, August). The tale of two clocks. In 2016 IEEE/ACM International Conference on Advances in Social Networks Analysis and Mining (ASONAM) (pp. 768-776). IEEE Computer Society.
  125. S2CID 241976819
    , retrieved 2022-03-21

Further reading

External links