Speech repetition
Speech repetition occurs when individuals speak the sounds that they have heard another person pronounce or say. In other words, it is the saying by one individual of the spoken vocalizations made by another individual. Speech repetition requires the person repeating the utterance to have the ability to map the sounds that they hear from the other person's oral pronunciation to similar places and manners of articulation in their own vocal tract.
Such speech imitation often occurs independently of speech comprehension such as in
In humans, the ability to map heard input vocalizations into motor output is highly developed because of the copying ability playing a critical role in children's rapid expansion of their
Properties
Automatic
Vocal imitation happens quickly: words can be repeated within 250-300
The automatic nature of speech repetition was noted by
Independent of speech
Vocal imitation arises in
The ability to repeat and imitate speech sounds occurs separately to that of normal speech. Speech shadowing provides evidence of a 'privileged' input/output speech loop that is distinct to the other components of the speech system.
Effector independent
Speech sounds can be imitatively mapped into vocal articulations in spite of vocal tract anatomy differences in size and shape due to
Diverse linguistic vocalizations
Vocal imitation occurs potentially in regard to a diverse range of phonetic units and types of vocalization. The world's languages use
Language acquisition
Vocabulary expansion
In 1874 Carl Wernicke proposed[26] that the ability to imitate speech plays a key role in language acquisition. This is now a widely researched issue in child development.[27][28][29][30][31] A study of 17,000 one and two word utterances made by six children between 18 months to 25 months found that, depending upon the particular infant, between 5% and 45% of their words might be mimicked.[27] These figures are minima since they concern only immediately heard words. Many words that may seem spontaneous are in fact delayed imitations heard days or weeks previously.[28] At 13 months children who imitate new words (but not ones they already know) show a greater increase in noun vocabulary at four months and non noun vocabulary at eight months.[29] A major predictor of vocabulary increase in both 20 months,[32] 24 months,[33] and older children between 4 and 8 years is their skill in repeating nonword phone sequences (a measure of mimicry and storage).[30][31] This is also the case with children with Down's syndrome .[34] The effect is larger than even age: in a study of 222 two-year-old children that had spoken vocabularies ranging between 3–601 words the ability to repeat nonwords accounted for 24% of the variance compared to 15% for age and 6% for gender (girls better than boys).[33]
Nonvocabulary expansion uses of imitation
Imitation provides the basis for making longer sentences than children could otherwise spontaneously make on their own.[35] Children analyze the linguistic rules, pronunciation patterns, and conversational pragmatics of speech by making monologues (often in crib talk) in which they repeat and manipulate in word play phrases and sentences previously overheard.[36] Many proto-conversations involve children (and parents) repeating what each other has said in order to sustain social and linguistic interaction. It has been suggested that the conversion of speech sound into motor responses helps aid the vocal "alignment of interactions" by "coordinating the rhythm and melody of their speech".[37] Repetition enables immigrant monolingual children to learn a second language by allowing them to take part in 'conversations'.[38] Imitation related processes aids the storage of overheard words by putting them into speech based short- and long-term memory.[39]
Language learning
The ability to repeat nonwords predicts the ability to learn second-language vocabulary.[40] A study found that adult polyglots performed better in short-term memory tasks such as repeating nonword vocalizations compared to nonpolyglots though both are otherwise similar in general intelligence, visuo-spatial short-term memory and paired-associate learning ability.[41] Language delay in contrast links to impairments in vocal imitation.[42]
Speech repetition and phones
Mechanism
Spoken words are sequences of motor movements organized around vocal tract gesture motor targets.[45] Vocalization due to this is copied in terms of the motor goals that organize it rather than the exact movements with which it is produced. These vocal motor goals are auditory. According to James Abbs[46] 'For speech motor actions, the individual articulatory movements would not appear to be controlled with regard to three- dimensional spatial targets, but rather with regard to their contribution to complex vocal tract goals such as resonance properties (e.g., shape, degree of constriction) and or aerodynamically significant variables'. Speech sounds also have duplicable higher-order characteristics such as rates and shape of modulations and rates and shape of frequency shifts.[47] Such complex auditory goals (which often link—though not always—to internal vocal gestures) are detectable from the speech sound which they create.
Neurology
Dorsal speech processing stream function
Two cortical processing streams exist: a ventral one which maps sound onto meaning, and a dorsal one, that maps sound onto motor representations. The dorsal stream projects from the posterior
Mirror neurons
Mirror neurons have been identified that both process the perception and production of motor movements. This is done not in terms of their exact motor performance but an inference of the intended motor goals with which it is organized.[52] Mirror neurons that both perceive and produce the motor movements of speech have been identified.[53] Speech is mirrored constantly into its articulations since speakers cannot know in advance that a word is unfamiliar and in need of repetition—which is only learnt after the opportunity to map it into articulations has gone. Thus, speakers if they are to incorporate unfamiliar words into their spoken vocabulary must by default map all spoken input.[54]
Sign language
Words in
Nonhuman animals
Birds
Birds learn their songs from those made by other birds. In several examples, birds show highly developed repetition abilities: the Sri Lankan Greater racket-tailed drongo (Dicrurus paradiseus) copies the calls of predators and the alarm signals of other birds[57] Albert's lyrebird (Menura alberti) can accurately imitate the satin bowerbird (Ptilonorhynchus violaceus),[58]
Research upon
Nonhuman mammals
- Bottlenose dolphins can show spontaneous vocal mimicry of computer-generated whistles.[61]
- Killer whales can mimic the barks of California sea lions.[62]
- Harbor seals can mimic in a speech-like manner one or more English words and phrases[63]
- imitate trunk sounds.[64]
- Lesser spear-nosed bat can learn their call structure from artificial playback.[65]
- An orangutan has spontaneously copied the whistles of humans.[66]
Apes
See also
- Alan Baddeley
- Auditory processing disorder
- Baddeley's model of working memory
- Conduction aphasia
- Developmental verbal dyspraxia
- Echoic memory
- Echolalia
- Language development
- Language acquisition
- Language-based learning disability
- Mirror neurons
- Mirroring (psychology)
- Motor cognition
- Motor theory of speech perception
- Origin of language
- Passive speakers
- Phonological development
- Pronunciation
- Second-language acquisition
- Short-term memory
- Speech perception
- Thematic coherence
- Transcortical motor aphasia
- Transcortical sensory aphasia
- Vocabulary growth
- Vocal learning
Footnotes
- S2CID 12662702.
- S2CID 4220775.
- ^ PMID 7421161.
- S2CID 20166301.
- PMID 1669805.
- ^ ISBN 978-90-272-1893-3
- PMID 7146899.
- PMID 8865648.
- S2CID 6925526.
- S2CID 143703500.
- PMID 390245.
- PMID 20273402.
- PMID 6582230.
- S2CID 35115074.
- ISBN 978-0-86377-005-0
- S2CID 6079313.
- PMID 6722512.
- PMID 11239078.
- ISBN 978-0-13-381764-5
- OCLC 156187572
- ^ Vocal traits also vary moreover when people get upper respiratory tract infections as the shape and size of sinus cavities is further changed with the swelling of mucous membranes.
- ^ S2CID 2113790.
- S2CID 40687020.
- PMID 15283067.
- S2CID 22042824.
- ^ Wernicke K. (1874). The aphasia symptom-complex. Breslau, Cohn and Weigert. Translated in: Eling P, editor. (1994). p. 69–89.Reader in the history of aphasia. Vol. 4. Amsterdam: John Benjamins: "The major tasks of the child in speech acquisition is mimicry of the spoken word". p76
- ^ .
- ^ ISBN 978-0-8164-9330-2
- ^ OCLC 89395784.
- ^ doi:10.1016/0749-596x(89)90044-2. Archived from the originalon 2012-08-17. Retrieved 2009-12-19.
- ^
- S2CID 18566002.
- ^ PMID 19017366.
- PMID 14982246.
- ISBN 978-0-387-96885-8
- ISBN 978-0-387-90860-1
- PMID 19277052. p. 201
- OCLC 4983571
- S2CID 20774241.
- .
- S2CID 19242688.
- PMID 8735439.
- S2CID 143189089.
- S2CID 39913744.
- ISBN 978-0-86377-005-0
- ISBN 978-0-89859-545-1
- ISBN 978-0-12-052750-2
- S2CID 635860.
- S2CID 1056984.
- PMID 11133789.
- PMID 16207930.
- PMID 11498058.
- PMID 19948355.
- PMID 20427741.
- ^ ISBN 978-0-262-66066-2
- S2CID 4414422.
- PMID 16618682.
- PMID 17148405.
- S2CID 19053313.
- ^ PMID 4833078.
- PMID 8375147.
- PMID 17148275.
- doi:10.1139/z85-157.
- S2CID 4369863.
- PMID 7827315.
- S2CID 708682.
- OCLC 1579444