Culture-bound syndrome

Source: Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia.

In

DSM-IV: Appendix I). Counterpart within the framework of ICD-10 (Chapter V) are the culture-specific disorders defined in Annex 2 of the Diagnostic criteria for research.[1]

More broadly, an

etiological grounds, it can be difficult to distinguish the causal contribution of culture upon disease from other environmental factors such as toxicity.[2]

Identification

A culture-specific syndrome is characterized by:[citation needed]

  • categorization as a disease in the culture (i.e., not a voluntary behaviour or false claim)
  • widespread familiarity in the culture
  • complete lack of familiarity or misunderstanding of the condition to people in other cultures
  • no objectively demonstrable biochemical or tissue abnormalities (signs)
  • recognition and treatment by the
    folk medicine
    of the culture

Some culture-specific syndromes involve somatic symptoms (pain or disturbed function of a body part), while others are purely behavioral. Some culture-bound syndromes appear with similar features in several cultures, but with locally specific traits, such as

penis panics
.

A culture-specific syndrome is not the same as a geographically localized disease with specific, identifiable, causal tissue abnormalities, such as kuru or sleeping sickness, or genetic conditions limited to certain populations. It is possible that a condition originally assumed to be a culture-bound behavioral syndrome is found to have a biological cause; from a medical perspective it would then be redefined into another nosological category.

Medical perspectives

The American Psychiatric Association states the following:[3]

The term culture-bound syndrome denotes recurrent, locality-specific patterns of aberrant behavior and troubling experience that may or may not be linked to a particular DSM-IV diagnostic category. Many of these patterns are indigenously considered to be "illnesses," or at least afflictions, and most have local names. Although presentations conforming to the major DSM-IV categories can be found throughout the world, the particular symptoms, course, and social response are very often influenced by local cultural factors. In contrast, culture-bound syndromes are generally limited to specific societies or culture areas and are localized, folk, diagnostic categories that frame coherent meanings for certain repetitive, patterned, and troubling sets of experiences and observations.

The term culture-bound syndrome is controversial since it reflects the different opinions of anthropologists and psychiatrists.[4] Anthropologists have a tendency to emphasize the relativistic and culture-specific dimensions of the syndromes, while physicians tend to emphasize the universal and neuropsychological dimensions.[5][6] Guarnaccia & Rogler (1999) have argued in favor of investigating culture-bound syndromes on their own terms, and believe that the syndromes have enough cultural integrity to be treated as independent objects of research.[7]

Guarnaccia and Rogler demonstrate the issues that occur when diagnosing cultural bound disorders using the DSM-IV. One of the key problems that arise is the "subsumption of culture bound syndromes into psychiatric categories",[7] which ultimately creates a medical hegemony and places the western perspective above that of other cultural and epistemological explanations of disease. The urgency for further investigation or reconsideration of the DSM-IV's authoritative power is emphasized, as the DSM becomes an international document for research and medical systems abroad. Guarnaccia and Rogler provide two research questions that must be considered, "firstly, how much do we know about the culture-bound syndromes for us to be able to fit them into standard classification; and secondly, whether such a standard and exhaustive classification in fact exists".[7]

It is suggested that the problematic nature of the DSM becomes evident when viewed as definitively conclusive. Questions are raised to whether culture-bound syndromes can be treated as discrete entities, or whether their symptoms are generalized and perceived as an amalgamation of previously diagnosed illnesses. If this is the case, then the DSM may be what Bruno Latour would define as "particular universalism". In that the Western medical system views itself to have a privileged insight into the true intelligence of nature, in contrast to the model provided by other cultural perspectives.[8]

Some studies suggest that culture-bound syndromes represent an acceptable way within a specific culture (and cultural context) among certain vulnerable individuals (i.e. an ataque de nervios at a funeral in Puerto Rico) to express distress in the wake of a traumatic experience.[9] A similar manifestation of distress when displaced into a North American medical culture may lead to a very different, even adverse outcome for a given individual and the individual's family.[10] The history and etymology of some syndromes such as brain-fog syndrome, have also been reattributed to 19th century Victorian Britain rather than West Africa.[11]

In 2013, the DSM 5 dropped the term culture-bound syndrome, preferring the new name "cultural concepts of distress".[12]

Cultural collusion between medical perspectives

Within the traditional Hmong culture, epilepsy (qaug dab peg) directly translates to "the spirit catches you and you fall down" which is said to be an evil spirit called a dab that captures one's soul and makes one ill. In this culture, individuals with seizures are seen to be blessed with a gift: an access point into the spiritual realm which no one else has been given.[13] In westernised society, epilepsy is recognized as a serious long-term brain condition that can have a major impairment on an individual's life. The way the illness is dealt with in Hmong culture is vastly different due to the high status epilepsy has in the culture, compared to individuals who have the condition in westernised societies. Individuals with epilepsy within the Hmong culture are a source of pride for their family.[14]

Another culture-bound illness is neurasthenia, which is a vaguely described medical ailment in Chinese culture that presents as lassitude, weariness, headaches, and irritability and is mostly linked to emotional disturbance. A report done in 1942 showed that 87% of patients diagnosed by Chinese psychiatrists as having neurasthenia could be reclassified as having major depression according to the DSM-3 criteria.[15] Another study conducted in Hong Kong showed that most patients selectively presented their symptoms according to what they perceived as appropriate and tended to only focus on somatic suffering, rather than the emotional problems they were facing.[16][17]

Globalisation

Globalisation is a process whereby information, cultures, jobs, goods, and services are spread across national borders.[18] This has had a powerful impact on the 21st century in many ways including through enriching cultural awareness across the globe. Greater level of cultural integration is occurring due to rapid industrialisation and globalisation, with cultures absorbing more influences from each other. As cultural awareness begins to increase between countries, there is a consideration into whether cultural bound syndromes will slowly lose their geographically bound nature and become commonly known syndromes that will then become internationally recognised. Anthropologist and psychiatrist Roland Littlewood makes the observation that these diseases are likely to vanish in an increasingly homogenous global culture in the face of globalisation (and industrialisation).[19] Depression, for example, was once only accepted in western societies; it is now recognised as a mental disorder in all parts of the world. In contrast to Eastern civilizations such as Taiwan, depression is still much more common in Western cultures like the United States. This could indicate that globalisation may have an impact on allowing disorders to be spread across borders, but these disorders may remain predominant in certain cultures.

DSM-IV-TR list

The fourth edition of Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders classifies the below syndromes as culture-bound syndromes:[20]

Name Geographical localization/populations
Running amok
Brunei, Singapore, Malaysia, Indonesia, Philippines, Timor-Leste
Ataque de nervios Latinos in the United States and Latin America
Bilis, cólera
Bouffée délirante France and French-speaking countries
Brain fag syndrome
West African
students
Dhat syndrome India
Falling-out, blacking out Southern United States and Caribbean
Ghost sickness Native American (Navajo, Muscogee/Creek)
Hwabyeong
Korean
Koro
Indonesian populations in Southeast Asia; Assam
; occasionally in the West
Latah Malaysia and Indonesia, as well as the Philippines (as
mali-mali, particularly among Tagalogs
)
Locura Latinos in the United States and Latin America
Mal de pelea
Puerto Rico
Evil eye Mediterranean; Hispanic populations and Ethiopia
Piblokto Arctic and subarctic Inuit populations
Zou huo ru mo
(Qigong psychotic reaction)
Han Chinese
Rootwork
Southern United States, Caribbean nations
Sangue dormido Cape Verde
Shenjing shuairuo Han Chinese
Shenkui, shen-k'uei Han Chinese
Shinbyeong
Koreans
Spell African American, White populations in the Southern United States and Ethiopia
Susto Latinos in the United States; Mexico, Central America and South America
Taijin kyofusho Japanese
Zār Ethiopia, Somalia, Egypt, Sudan, Iran, and other North African and Middle Eastern societies

DSM-5 list

The fifth edition of Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders classifies the below syndromes as cultural concepts of distress, a closely related concept:[21]

Name Geographical localization/populations
Ataque de nervios Latin America
Dhat syndrome India
Khyâl cap
Cambodian
Ghost sickness Native American
Kufungisisa Zimbabwe
Maladi moun Haiti
Shenjing shuairuo Han Chinese
Susto Latinos in the United States; Mexico, Central and South America
Taijin kyofusho Japanese

ICD-10 list

The 10th revision of the

International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems (ICD) classifies the below syndromes as culture-specific disorders:[1]

Name Geographical localization/populations
Amok
Southeast Asian Austronesians
Dhat syndrome (dhātu), shen-k'uei, jiryan India; Taiwan
Koro
, suk yeong, jinjin bemar
Southeast Asia, India, China
Latah Malaysia and Indonesia
Ataque de nervios Mexico, Central and South America
Pa-leng (frigophobia
)
Taiwan; Southeast Asia
Pibloktoq (Arctic hysteria) Inuit living within the Arctic Circle
Susto, espanto Mexico, Central and South America
anthropophobia
)
Japan
Ufufuyane
, saka
Kenya; southern Africa (among Bantu, Zulu, and affiliated groups)
Uqamairineq [ru] Inuit living within the Arctic Circle
Fear of
Windigo
Indigenous people of north-east America

Other examples

Though "the ethnocentric bias of Euro-American psychiatrists has led to the idea that culture-bound syndromes are confined to non-Western cultures",[22] within the contiguous United States, the

kaolin, a type of clay, has been proposed as a culture-bound syndrome observed in African Americans in the rural South, particularly in areas in which the mining of kaolin is common.[23]

In South Africa, among the Xhosa people, the syndrome of amafufunyana is commonly used to describe those believed to be possessed by demons or other malevolent spirits. Traditional healers in the culture usually perform exorcisms in order to drive off these spirits. Upon investigating the phenomenon, researchers found that many of the people claimed to be affected by the syndrome exhibited the traits and characteristics of schizophrenia.[24][25]

Some researchers have suggested that both premenstrual syndrome (PMS) and the more severe premenstrual dysphoric disorder (PMDD), which have currently unknown physical mechanisms,[26][27][28] are Western culture-bound syndromes.[29][30] However, this is controversial.[29]

Tarantism is an expression of mass psychogenic illness documented in Southern Italy since the 11th century.[31]

Morgellons is a rare self-diagnosed skin condition reported primarily in white populations in the United States.[32] It has been described by a journalist as "a socially transmitted disease over the Internet".[33]

umbrella term nature as neurological condition also results in diagnosing neurotic patients as neurological ones,[34][35]
in effect substituting possible psychiatric stigma with culture-bound syndrome disguised as a neurological condition.

Refugee children in Sweden have been known to fall into coma-like states on learning their families will be deported. The condition, known in Swedish as uppgivenhetssyndrom, or resignation syndrome, is believed to only exist among the refugee population in Sweden, where it has been prevalent since the early part of the 21st century. In a 130-page report on the condition commissioned by the government and published in 2006, a team of psychologists, political scientists, and sociologists hypothesized that it was a culture-bound syndrome.[36]

A startle disorder similar to latah, called imu [ja] (sometimes spelled imu:), is found among Ainu people, both Sakhalin Ainu and Hokkaido Ainu.[37][38]

A condition similar to piblokto, called menerik [ru] (sometimes meryachenie), is found among Yakuts, Yukaghirs, and Evenks living in Siberia.[39]

The trance-like violent behavior of the

Viking age berserkers – behavior that disappeared with the arrival of Christianity – has been described as a culture-bound syndrome.[40]

See also

References

  1. ^ a b Diagnostic criteria for research, p. 213–225 (WHO 1993)
  2. . Retrieved 25 August 2013.
  3. ^ Perry, S. (2012, 13 January). The controversy over 'culture-bound' mental illnesses. Retrieved 27 January 2013 from MinnPost.
  4. S2CID 2159608
    .
  5. ^ .
  6. ^ Latour, Bruno (1991). We Have Never Been Modern (PDF). Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press. p. 105.
  7. S2CID 33348909
    .
  8. .
  9. .
  10. .
  11. ^ "When Epilepsy Goes By Another Name". Epilepsy Foundation. Retrieved 27 August 2022.
  12. S2CID 231201804
    .
  13. .
  14. .
  15. , retrieved 27 August 2022
  16. .
  17. .
  18. .
  19. ^ Grigsby RK, Thyer BA, Waller RJ, Johnston GA Jr. (February 1992). "Chalk Eating in Middle Georgia: A Culture-Bound Syndrome of Pica?" South Med J. (2): 190–192.
  20. S2CID 23545601
    .
  21. .
  22. .
  23. .
  24. .
  25. ^ a b Alia Offman, Peggy J. Kleinplatz (2004). "Does PMDD Belong in the DSM? Challenging the Medicalization of Women's Bodies". Archived 2011-06-28 at the Wayback Machine Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality, Vol. 13.
  26. S2CID 30238189
    .
  27. .
  28. .
  29. ^ "Pathogens & People: Internet helps spread delusion that Morgellons a disease". Hometown Annapolis. 9 July 2011. Archived from the original on 9 July 2011. Retrieved 8 August 2020.
  30. S2CID 70674537
    . Retrieved 2 October 2016.
  31. ]
  32. ^ Rachel Aviv (3 April 2017). "The Trauma of Facing Deportation". The New Yorker. Retrieved 1 July 2017.
  33. .
  34. .
  35. .
  36. .

Further reading

External links