Self-awareness
Part of a series on |
The Self |
---|
Constructs |
Theories |
Processes |
Value judgment |
As applied to activities |
Interpersonal |
Social |
Politics |
In
.Biology
Mirror neurons
Researchers are investigating which part of the
Body
Bodily (self-)awareness is related to proprioception and visualization.[6] In health and medicine, body awareness refers to a person's ability to direct their focus on various internal sensations accurately. Both proprioception and interoception allow individuals to be consciously aware of multiple sensations.[7] Proprioception allows individuals and patients to focus on sensations in their muscles and joints, posture, and balance, while interoception is used to determine sensations of the internal organs, such as fluctuating heartbeat, respiration, lung pain, or satiety. Over-acute body-awareness, under-acute body-awareness, and distorted body-awareness are symptoms present in a variety of health disorders and conditions, such as obesity, anorexia nervosa, and chronic joint pain.[8] For example, a distorted perception of satiety is present in a patient suffering from anorexia nervosa.
Psychology
Self-awareness has been called "arguably the most fundamental issue in psychology, from both a developmental and an evolutionary perspective."[9]
Self-awareness theory, developed by Duval and Wicklund in their 1972 landmark book A theory of objective self awareness, states that when we focus on ourselves, we evaluate and compare our current behavior to our internal standards and values. This elicits a state of objective self-awareness. We become self-conscious as objective evaluators of ourselves.[10] Self-awareness should not be confused with self-consciousness.[11] Various emotional states are intensified by self-awareness. However, some people may seek to increase their self-awareness through these outlets[specify]. People are more likely to align their behavior with their standards when they are made self-aware. People are negatively affected[how?] if they do not live up to their personal standards. Various environmental cues and situations induce awareness of the self, such as mirrors, an audience, or being videotaped or recorded. These cues also increase the accuracy of personal memory.[12]
In one of
Albert Bandura's theory of self-efficacy describes "the belief in one's capabilities to organize and execute the courses of action required to manage prospective situations." A person's belief in their ability to succeed sets the stage for how they think, behave, and feel. Someone with a strong self-efficacy, for example, views challenges as tasks to engage in, and is not easily discouraged by setbacks. Such a person is aware of their flaws and abilities and chooses to utilize these qualities to the best of their ability. Someone with a weak sense of self-efficacy evades challenges and quickly feels discouraged by setbacks. They may not be aware of these negative reactions and therefore, may not be prompted to change their attitude. This concept is central to Bandura's social cognitive theory, "which emphasizes the role of observational learning, social experience, and reciprocal determinism in the development of personality."[15][unreliable source?]
Human development
Developmental stages
Individuals become conscious of themselves through the development of self-awareness.[9] This particular type of self-development pertains to becoming conscious of one's body and one's state of mind—including thoughts, actions, ideas, feelings, and interactions with others.[16] "Self-awareness does not occur suddenly through one particular behavior: it develops gradually through a succession of different behaviors all of which relate to the self."[17] The monitoring of one's mental states is called metacognition and is considered to be an indicator that there is some concept of the self.[18]
According to Philippe Rochat, there are five levels of self-awareness that unfold in early human development and six potential prospects ranging from "Level 0" (having no self-awareness) advancing complexity to "Level 5" (explicit self-awareness):[9]
- Level 0—Confusion: The person is unaware of any mirror reflection or the mirroring itself; they perceive a mirror image as an extension of their environment.
- Level 1—Differentiation: The individual realizes the mirror is able to reflect things. They see that what is in the mirror is of a different nature from what is surrounding them. At this level they can differentiate between their own movement in the mirror and the movement of the surrounding environment.
- Level 2—Situation: The individual can link the movements on the mirror to what is perceived within their own body.
- Level 3—Identification: An individual can now see that what's in the mirror is not another person but actually them.
- Level 4—Permanence: The individual is able to identify the self in previous pictures looking different or younger. A "permanent self" is now experienced.
- Level 5—Self-consciousness or "meta" self-awareness: At this level not only is the self seen from a first person view but it is realized that it is also seen from a third person's view. A person who develops self consciousness begins to understand they can be in the mind of others: for instance, how they are seen from a public standpoint.
Infancy and early childhood
When a human infant comes into the world, they have no concept of what is around them, nor the significance of others around them.[19]: 46 At first "the infant cannot recognize its own face".[19]: 46 At only a few months old, infants know the relationship between the proprioceptive and visual information they receive.[20] This is called "first-person self-awareness".
By the time an average toddler reaches 18–24 months, they discover themselves and recognize their own reflection in the mirror,[21][22] however the exact age varies with differing socioeconomic levels and differences relating to culture and parenting.[23] Those who reach this level of awareness recognize that they see themselves, for instance, seeing dirt on their face in the reflection and then touching their face to wipe it off. Soon after toddlers become reflectively self-aware, they begin to recognize their bodies as physical objects in time and space that interact and impact other objects. For instance, a toddler placed on a blanket, when asked to hand someone the blanket, will recognize that they need to get off it to be able to lift it.[20] This is the final stage of body self-awareness and is called objective self-awareness.
By 18 months of age, an infant can communicate their name to others, and upon being shown a picture they are in, they can identify themselves. By two years old, they also usually acquire gender category and age categories, saying things such as "I am a girl, not a boy" and "I am a baby or child, not a grownup". As an infant moves to middle childhood and onwards to adolescence, they develop more advanced levels of self-awareness and self-description.[19] By the age of 24 months, the toddler will observe and relate their own actions to actions of other people and the surrounding environment.[21]
As a preschooler, they begin to give much more specific details about things, instead of generalizing. At this age,[specify] the child is in what Jean Piaget names the pre operational stage of development. The infant is very inaccurate at judging themselves. For example, an infant at this stage will not associate that they are strong with their ability to cross the jungle gym at their school, nor will they associate the fact that they can solve a math problem with their ability to count.[19]
Around school age, a child's awareness of their memory transitions into a sense of their self. At this stage, a child begins to develop interests, likes, and dislikes. This transition enables a person's awareness of their past, present, and future to grow as they remember their conscious experiences more often.[21]
Adolescence
One becomes conscious of one's emotions during adolescence. Most children are aware of emotions such as shame, guilt, pride, and embarrassment by the age of two, but do not fully understand how those emotions affect their life.[24][page needed] By age 13, children become more in touch with these emotions and begin to apply them to their lives. Many adolescents display happiness and self-confidence around friends, but hopelessness and anger around parents due to the fear of being a disappointment. Teenagers may feel intelligent and creative around teachers, while they may feel shy, uncomfortable, and nervous around people they are not familiar with.[25]
As children reach adolescence, their acute sense of emotion has widened into a meta-cognitive state in which mental health issues can become more prevalent due to heightened emotional and social development.[26] Self-awareness training may reduce anger management issues and reduce aggressive tendencies in adolescents.[27]
In adolescent development, self-awareness has a more complex emotional context than in the early childhood phase. Elements can include self-image, self-concept, and self-consciousness among other traits that relate to Rochat's final level of self awareness, however self-awareness remains a distinct concept.[28]
Measurement
There are two common methods used to measure how severe an individual's lack of self-awareness is. The Patient Competency Rating Scale (PCRS) evaluates self-awareness in patients who have endured a traumatic brain injury.[29] PCRS is a 30-item self-report instrument which asks the subject to use a 5-point Likert scale to rate his or her degree of difficulty in a variety of tasks and functions. Independently, relatives or significant others who know the patient well are also asked to rate the patient on each of the same behavioral items. The difference between the relatives' and patient's perceptions is considered an indirect measure of impaired self-awareness. The limitations of this experiment rest on the answers of the relatives. Results of their answers can lead to a bias. In addition, in terms of treatment effectiveness, this experimental method works better for individuals with moderate and severe traumatic brain injury, but less effectively for those with mild brain injury. This is because, although individuals with mild brain injury fall within the normal range in psychological test results, they may still experience cognitive difficulties. Therefore, these everyday difficulties are more apparent in subjective assessments and behavioral observations.[30] These two limitations prompted a second method of testing a patient's self-awareness. Simply asking a patient why they are in the hospital or what is wrong with their body can give compelling answers as to what they see and are analyzing.[31]
Disorders
Anosognosia
The medical term for not being aware of one's deficits is anosognosia, or more commonly known as a lack of insight. Having a lack of awareness raises the risks of treatment and service nonadherence.[32] A wide variety of disorders are associated with anosognosia. For example, patients who are blind from cortical lesions might in fact be unaware that they are blind and may state that they do not suffer from any visual disturbances. Individuals with aphasia may be unaware of certain speech errors.[33] Individuals who suffer from Alzheimer's disease lack awareness; this deficiency becomes more intense throughout their disease.[34] A key issue with this disorder is that people who do have anosognosia and suffer from certain illnesses may not be aware of them, which ultimately leads them to put themselves in dangerous positions.[33]
Autism spectrum disorder

A 2008 study suggested that self-awareness in
Schizophrenia
Schizophrenia as a disease state is characterized by severe cognitive dysfunction and it is uncertain to what extent patients are aware of this deficiency. Medalia and Lim (2004) investigated patients' awareness of their cognitive deficit in the areas of attention, nonverbal memory, and verbal memory.[38] Results from this study (N=185) revealed large discrepancy in patients' assessment of their cognitive functioning relative to the assessment of their clinicians. Though it is impossible to access one's consciousness and truly understand what a schizophrenic believes, regardless in this study, patients were not aware of their cognitive dysfunctional reasoning.
A 1993 study suggests a correlation exists between patient insight, compliance, and disease progression. Patients with poor insight are less likely to be compliant with treatment and are more likely to have a poorer prognosis. Patients with hallucinations sometimes experience positive symptoms, which can include delusions of reference, thought insertion/withdrawal, thought broadcast, delusions of persecution, and grandiosity.[39]
Non-human animals

"Mirror tests" have been done on chimpanzees, elephants, dolphins and magpies. During the test, the experimenter looks for the animals to undergo four stages:[40]
- social response (behaving toward the reflection as they would toward another animal of their species)
- physical mirror inspection
- repetitive mirror testing behavior, and
- the mark test, which involves the animals spontaneously touching a mark on their body that would have been difficult to see without the mirror
The red-spot technique, created by Gordon G. Gallup,[41] studies self-awareness in primates. This technique places a red odorless spot on an anesthetized primate's forehead. The spot is placed on the forehead so it can only be seen through a mirror. Once the primate awakens, its independent movements toward the spot after it sees its reflection in a mirror are observed.
Apes
Chimpanzees and other apes—extensively studied species—are most similar to humans, with the most convincing findings and straightforward evidence of self-awareness in animals.[43] During the red-spot technique, after looking in the mirror, chimpanzees used their fingers to touch the red dot on their forehead and, after touching the red dot they would smell their fingertips.[44] "Animals that can recognize themselves in mirrors can conceive of themselves," says Gallup.
Dolphins
Dolphins were put to a similar test and achieved the same results. Diana Reiss, a psycho-biologist at the New York Aquarium discovered that bottlenose dolphins can recognize themselves in mirrors.[45]
Elephants
In a 2006 study, one elephant out of three passed the mirror test.[40]
Magpies
Researchers also used the mark or mirror tests to study the magpie's self-awareness.[46] As a majority of birds are blind below the beak, Prior et al. marked the birds' neck with three different colors: red, yellow, and black (as an imitation, as magpies are originally black). When placed in front of a mirror, the birds with red and yellow spots began scratching at their necks, signaling the understanding of something different being on their bodies. During one trial with a mirror and a mark, three of the five magpies showed at least one example of self-directed behavior. The magpies explored the mirror by moving toward it and looking behind it. One of the magpies, Harvey, during several trials would pick up objects, pose, do some wing-flapping, all in front of the mirror with the objects in his beak. This represents a sense of self-awareness; knowing what is going on within himself and in the present. The authors suggest that self-recognition in birds and mammals may be a case of convergent evolution, where similar evolutionary pressures result in similar behaviors or traits, although they arrive at them via different routes.[47]
A few slight occurrences of behavior towards the magpie's own body happened in the trial with the black mark and the mirror. The authors of this study suggest that the black mark may have been slightly visible on the black feathers. "This is an indirect support for the interpretation that the behavior towards the mark region was elicited by seeing the own body in the mirror in conjunction with an unusual spot on the body."[46]
There was a clear contrast between the behaviors of the magpies when a mirror was present versus absent. In the no-mirror trials, a non-reflective gray plate was swapped in the same size and position as the mirror. There were not any mark-directed self-behaviors when the mark was present, in color or in black.[46] The results show that magpies understand that a mirror image represents their own body; magpies have self-awareness.
Other uses
Plants
Science fiction
In
Collective self-awareness
Alongside self-awareness seen as a personal capability, the same term may be applied to the self-awareness of groups or
Robotics
It has been proposed that robots which use internal models to simulate their own actions could be classified as functionally self-aware. This definition does not concern itself with the philosophical question of whether these robots really are self-aware.[52]
See also
References
- ^ "self-awareness". Merriam-Webster. September 15, 2023.
- ^ "The roots of human self-awareness | Iowa Now - The University of Iowa". now.uiowa.edu. Retrieved January 21, 2025.
- .
- ISBN 978-1-934115-34-3.
- ^ Ramachandran, V.S. (2009). "Self Awareness: The Last Frontier". Edge.org. Retrieved July 26, 2011.
- ^ de Vignemont, Frédérique (July 8, 2020) [2011-08-09]. "Bodily Awareness". Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy.
- PMID 19440300.
- S2CID 28010263.
- ^ S2CID 10241157.
- OCLC 643552644.[page needed]
- ISBN 978-0-203-41898-7.[page needed]
- ISBN 978-1-4613-5579-3.
- ISBN 978-1-134-69601-7.[page needed]
- .
- ^ Cherry, Kendra (July 5, 2019), Self Efficacy and Why Believing in Yourself Matters
- ^ Geangu, Elena (March 2008). "Notes on self awareness development in early infancy" (PDF). Cognitie, Creier, Comportament / Cognition, Brain, Behavior. 12 (1): 103–113. ProQuest 201571751.
- .
- PMID 25108418.
- ^ ISBN 9780203767696.
- ^ .
- ^ S2CID 985947.
- PMID 17883440.
- S2CID 18326326.
- ^ Zeanah, Charles (2009). Handbook of Infant Mental Health. New York: Guilford Press.
- ISBN 9781572304321.
- .
- .
- .
- PMID 10079045.
- ISSN 1380-3395.
- .
- ^ Xavier, Amador. "Anosognosia (Lack of Insight) Fact Sheet". Archived from the original on November 1, 2015. Retrieved November 26, 2014.
- ^ PMID 9854262.
- PMID 1533.
- PMID 18958161.
- ^ PMID 21207316.
- S2CID 22219711.
- S2CID 19673249.
- PMID 8494061.
- ^ PMID 17075063.
- S2CID 10070614.
- ISBN 9780511819001. Archived from the original(PDF) on May 17, 2016. Retrieved November 4, 2023.
- .
- ISBN 978-0-262-52322-6.
- ^ Tennesen, Michael (2003). "Do Dolphins Have a Sense of Self?". National Wildlife. World Edition. Archived from the original on August 9, 2010.
- ^ PMID 18715117.
- ^ Alison, Motluk. "Mirror test shows magpies aren't so bird-brained". New Scientist. Retrieved November 26, 2014.
- PMID 26311669.
- ISBN 978-0-19-517452-6
- , accessed on 14 August 2024
- ^ Pope Paul VI, Ecclesiam Suam, paragraph 26, published on 6 August 1964, accessed on 14 August 2024
- ISBN 978-1-78326-417-9.
External links
- Ashley, Greg; Reiter-Palmon, Roni (September 1, 2012). "Self-Awareness and the Evolution of Leaders: The Need for a Better Measure of Self-Awareness". Journal of Behavioral and Applied Management. 14 (1): 2–17. .
- Mograbi, Daniel C.; Hall, Simon; Arantes, Beatriz; Huntley, Jonathan (2024). "The cognitive neuroscience of self-awareness: Current framework, clinical implications, and future research directions". WIREs Cognitive Science. 15 (2): e1670. PMID 38043919.