Sleep spindle

Sleep spindles are bursts of
Research supports that spindles (sometimes referred to as "sigma bands" or "sigma waves") play an essential role in both sensory processing and long term memory consolidation. Until recently, it was believed that each sleep spindle oscillation peaked at the same time throughout the neocortex. It was determined that oscillations sweep across the neocortex in circular patterns around the neocortex, peaking in one area, and then a few milliseconds later in an adjacent area. It has been suggested that this spindle organization allows for neurons to communicate across cortices. The time scale at which the waves travel at is the same speed it takes for neurons to communicate with each other.[8] Doubts, however, remain whether a link exists between sleep spindles and memory with a recent meta-review of 53 studies concluding that "there is no relationship between sleep spindles and memory, and thus it is unlikely that sleep spindles are indeed generally implicated in learning and plasticity".[9]
Although the function of sleep spindles is unclear, it is believed that they actively participate in the consolidation of overnight declarative memory through the reconsolidation process. The density of spindles has been shown to increase after extensive learning of declarative memory tasks and the degree of increase in stage 2 spindle activity correlates with memory performance.
Among other functions, spindles facilitate somatosensory development, thalamocortical sensory gating, synaptic plasticity, and offline memory consolidation.[10] Sleep spindles closely modulate interactions between the brain and its external environment; they essentially moderate responsiveness to sensory stimuli during sleep.[11] Recent research has revealed that spindles distort the transmission of auditory information to the cortex; spindles isolate the brain from external disturbances during sleep.[12] Another study found that re-exposure to olfactory cues during sleep initiate reactivation, an essential part of long term memory consolidation that improves later recall performance.[13] Spindles generated in the thalamus have been shown to aid sleeping in the presence of disruptive external sounds. A correlation has been found between the amount of brainwave activity in the thalamus and a sleeper's ability to maintain tranquility.[14] Spindles play an essential role in both sensory processing and long term memory consolidation because they are generated in the TRN.
During sleep, these spindles are seen in the brain as a burst of activity immediately following muscle twitching. Researchers think the brain, particularly in the young, is learning about what nerves control what specific muscles when asleep.[15][16]
Sleep spindle activity has furthermore been found to be associated with the integration of new information into existing knowledge[17] as well as directed remembering and forgetting (fast sleep spindles).[18]
During
Research is currently underway to develop a web-based automatic sleep spindle detection system by using machine learning techniques. The results of the present study show that the automatic sleep spindle detection system has great potential in practical application.[24]
Evolution
No sleep spindles have been observed in reptiles and birds so far.[25][26][27] In most mammals sleep spindles were described at face value, but the existence of pseudo-spindles[6] invites uncertainty around these observations. Spindle-like oscillations, that show additional analogies (e.g. apparent involvement in learning or thalamic dependence) are currently only known from humans, rats, mice, cats, and dogs.[7] In these species spindles invariantly oscillate between 9 and 16 Hz, with minor variations (e.g. 7–14 Hz in the cat[28]). Clearly distinct frontal and posterior sleep spindles (i.e. "slow" and "fast") were only confirmed outside humans in rats[29] and dogs.[30]
Sex differences
Sleep spindles play a crucial role in
Humanlike sex differences in sleep spindle activity and sleep-dependent learning were observed in dogs.[30][34] A direct link to specific sexual hormones could not be established, but the effects were stronger in intact animals.[30]
Females tend to have 0.16 more sleep spindles per minute than males[35] ( i.e. roughly 9–10 more over an hour's time). A female advantage has been found for episodic, emotional, and spatial memories as well as recognition of odours, faces, and pictures.[36] These differences are believed to be due to hormonal influence, especially that of estrogen. The female sex hormone estrogen primarily influences sexual maturation and reproduction, but has also been found to facilitate other brain functions, including cognition and memory. On verbal tasks where women scored higher than men, women scored higher during the mid-luteal phase, when females have higher estrogen levels, when compared to the menstrual phase.[31] A recent study found that local brain estrogen production within cognitive circuits may be important for the acquisition and consolidation of memories.[37] Recent experiments concerning the relationship between oestrogen and the process of offline memory consolidation have also focused on sleep spindles. Genzel and colleagues determined that there was a menstrual effect on declarative and motor performance, meaning that females in the mid-luteal phase (high estrogen) performed higher than the other female participants.[33] Females in the luteal phase were also the only participants to experience an increase in spindles after learning, which led to the conclusion that the effect of the menstrual cycle may be mediated by spindles and female hormones.[33]
References
- ISBN 978-1-4557-7051-9. Retrieved 5 June 2019.
- OCLC 2518321.
- PMID 14573378.
- PMID 9056712.
- S2CID 26291991.
- ^ a b Gottesmann, C. (1996). The transition from slow-wave sleep to paradoxical sleep: evolving facts and concepts of the neurophysiological processes underlying the intermediate stage of sleep. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 20, 367–387.
- ^ a b Iotchev, I. B., & Kubinyi, E. (2021). Shared and unique features of mammalian sleep spindles–insights from new and old animal models. Biological Reviews, 96(3), 1021-1034.
- PMID 27855061.
- PMID 38608930.
- PMID 22591117.
- S2CID 206658010.
- PMID 21896732.
- S2CID 22066368.
- PMID 20692606.
- ^ Dingfelder SF (January 2006). "To sleep, perchance to twitch". Monitor. 37 (1). American Psychological Association: 51.
- ^ Harney É (April 1, 2009). "Wiring your brain at college – a new perspective on sleep". Blog at WordPress.com. Archived from the original on 2010-06-19.
- PMID 20980591.
- PMID 21459838.
- PMID 17329474.
- S2CID 30679161.
- PMID 30853050.
- S2CID 206608057.
- PMID 24562424.
- S2CID 236141540.
- ^ Rattenborg, N. C., Martinez-Gonzalez, D., Roth, T. C. & Pravosudov, V. V. (2011). Hippocampal memory consolidation during sleep: a comparison of mammals and birds. Biological Reviews 86, 658–691.
- ^ Shein-Idelson, M., Ondracek, J. M., Liaw, H. P., Reiter, S. & Laurent, G. (2016). Slow waves, sharp waves, ripples, and REM in sleeping dragons. Science 352, 590–595.
- ^ van der Meij, J., Martinez-Gonzalez, D., Beckers, G. J. L. & Rattenborg, N. C. (2019). Intra-"cortical" activity during avian non-REM and REM sleep: variant and invariant traits between birds and mammals. Sleep 42, zsy230.
- ^ Steriade, M., & Llinás, R. R. (1988). The functional states of the thalamus and the associated neuronal interplay. Physiological reviews, 68(3), 649-742.
- ^ Terrier, G., & Gottesmann, C. L. (1978). Study of cortical spindles during sleep in the rat. Brain research bulletin, 3(6), 701-706.
- ^ a b c Iotchev, I. B., Kis, A., Turcsán, B., Tejeda Fernández de Lara, D. R., Reicher, V., & Kubinyi, E. (2019). Age-related differences and sexual dimorphism in canine sleep spindles. Scientific reports, 9(1), 1-11.
- ^ PMID 10450590.
- S2CID 15133827.
- ^ S2CID 19797939.
- ^ Iotchev, I. B., Kis, A., Bódizs, R., van Luijtelaar, G., & Kubinyi, E. (2017). EEG transients in the sigma range during non-REM sleep predict learning in dogs. Scientific reports, 7(1), 1-11.
- PMID 28649997.
- PMID 15504424.
- PMID 27453921.