Genetics of aggression
The field of psychology has been greatly influenced by the study of genetics.[1] Decades of research have demonstrated that both genetic and environmental factors play a role in a variety of behaviors in humans and animals (e.g. Grigorenko & Sternberg, 2003). The genetic basis of aggression, however, remains poorly understood. Aggression is a multi-dimensional concept, but it can be generally defined as behavior that inflicts pain or harm on another.[2]
The genetic-developmental theory states that individual differences in a continuous
History
Past thoughts on genetic factors influencing aggression, specifically in regard to sex chromosomes, tended to seek answers from
The XYY genotype first gained wide notoriety in 1968 when it was raised as a part of a defense in two murder trials in Australia and France. In the United States, five attempts to use the XYY genotype as a defense were unsuccessful—in only one case in 1969 was it allowed to go to a jury—which rejected it.[7]
Results from several decades of long-term follow-up of scores of unselected XYY males identified in eight international newborn chromosome screening studies in the 1960s and 1970s have replaced pioneering but
Heritability
As with other topics in
- genesassociated with certain behaviors such as aggression.
- Mechanism experiments – studies to determine the biological mechanisms that lead certain genes to influence types of behavior like aggression.
- Genetic behavior twin studies and adoption studies.
These three main experimental types are used in animal studies, studies testing heritability and molecular genetics, and gene/environment interaction studies. Recently, important links between aggression and genetics have been studied and the results are allowing scientists to better understand the connections.[10]
Selective breeding
The
Mouse studies
Oxidative stress arises as a result of excess production of reactive oxygen species in relation to defense mechanisms, including the action of antioxidants such as superoxide dismutase 1 (SOD1). Knockout of the Sod1 gene was experimentally introduced in male mice leading to impaired antioxidant defense.[14] These mice were designated (Sod1-/-). The Sod1-/- male mice proved to be more aggressive than both heterozygous knockout males (Sod1+/-) that were 50% deficient in SOD1, and wild-type males (Sod1+/+).[14] The basis for the association of oxidative stress with increased aggression has not yet been determined.
Biological mechanisms
Experiments designed to study biological mechanisms are utilized when exploring how
Molecular genetics
A number of
Human behavior genetics
In determining if a
DAT1 is a gene that regulates dopamine levels in the brain. This study revealed that variations in the DAT1 gene were correlated with higher levels of aggression. Some people that have variations of the DAT1 gene exhibit more aggressive behaviors. DAT2 controls how the brain responds to dopamine. Certain combinations of DAT1 and DAT2 genes were linked to an increase or decrease in aggressive behaviors. While the relationship remains unclear, there is certainly a correlation between certain forms of DAT1 and DAT2 and varying combinations of each. [20] Changes in these genes can cause changes in neurotransmitter levels. When typical neurotransmitter levels change, other bodily behaviors are also affected. Examples of other functions that are impacted are intelligence, mood, and memory. [21]
Twin studies
Twin studies are studies typically conducted comparing
Adoption studies
Adoption studies are a specific research designs that involve comparing traits between an adopted child and their biological and adoptive parents. These experiments aim to assess both biological and environmental factors that may be attributed to aggression. Adoption studies have shown stronger similarities between adopted children and their biological parents, indicating that there is a genetic component at play. However, children have also shown similarities with their adoptive parents, indicating that there are environmental factors as well. These studies further support the complex nature of aggression by proving that there are both biological and environmental factors involved. More research needs to be conducted to truly prove the causes of aggression. [22]
Genetics of aggression over time
Over time, studies pertaining to the genetics of aggression have improved, and become an interesting research topic for those looking for research opportunities. Experiments started in 1963 with the
Male vs female aggression
Aggression can manifest in different ways between biological males and females. A study evaluated these differences by using EEG and ECG to monitor neurobiological responses to aggravating stimuli. It was shown that anger and physical aggression was much greater in men than women. Men also scored higher on a scale regarding reactive aggression. The EEG test also supported the idea that women show weaker responses regarding aggression. It was also shown that men and women follow different pathways in the brain when aggression is invoked, although further studies are needed in order to solidify these findings. [23]
Environmental factors
Aggression can have many causes, including environmental factors. Environmental factors include any physical, chemical, and biological environmental factors that can influence aggression. Studies have shown that neighborhood greenspace can vastly reduce aggressive behaviors in children and adolescents. One proposed explanation for this finding is that greenspace has been proven to reduce stress and depression. HIgher stress and depression levels in parents have been shown to increase aggressive behaviors in children. By lowering stress and depression in parents, children are more likely to show a decrease in aggressive behaviors. In addition, greenspace promotes participation in physical activity and social involvement. Another study revealed that low-frequency, high-intensity, and continuous noises were associated with more aggressive behaviors. [24]
See also
Notes
- S2CID 17398272.
- PMID 27061441.
- ^ ISBN 1-59385-110-3.
- PMID 4727279.
- ^ Court Brown, W.M. (1967). Human Population Cytogenetics. Amsterdam: North-Holland Publishing Company.
- ISBN 0-7216-5160-7.
- ISBN 0-471-95719-4.
- ISBN 0-443-06434-2.
- ISBN 0-8018-7928-0.
- ^ ISBN 0-19-516876-3.
- ISBN 90-286-2851-7.
- ^ Southwick, Charles H. (1970). Animal Aggression: Selected Readings. London: Litton Educational Publishing Inc.
- ^ ISBN 0-471-95719-4.
- ^ PMID 25524980.
- ^ Stangor, Charles; Walinga, Jennifer (2019-06-28). "4.4 Is Personality More Nature or More Nurture? Behavioural and Molecular Genetics". Introduction to Psychology.
- ^ S2CID 14070721.
- PMID 24193094.
- PMID 15964153.
- PMID 25419014.
- PMID 24193094.
- ^ "The genetics of violent behavior". The Jackson Laboratory. Retrieved 2024-12-02.
- ^ "Genetic Factors - Psychology: AQA A Level". senecalearning.com. Retrieved 2024-12-02.
- PMID 30636903.
- PMID 27343886.
References
- Grigorenko, E.L. & Sternberg, R.J. (2003). The nature nurture issue. In A. Slater & G. Bremner (Eds.), An introduction to developmental psychology. Malden, MA: Blackwell.
- Pomp, D. (2010). Genomic mapping of social behavior traits in a F2 cross derived from mice selectively bred for high aggression. BCM Genetics, 11:113. doi:10.1186/1471-2156-11-113