Pain in crustaceans

Source: Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia.
Close up of the head of a live lobster

There is a scientific debate which questions whether crustaceans experience pain. It is a complex mental state, with a distinct perceptual quality but also associated with suffering, which is an emotional state. Because of this complexity, the presence of pain in an animal, or another human for that matter, cannot be determined unambiguously using observational methods, but the conclusion that animals experience pain is often inferred on the basis of likely presence of phenomenal consciousness which is deduced from comparative brain physiology as well as physical and behavioural reactions.[1][2]

Definitions of pain vary, but most involve the ability of the nervous system to detect and reflexively react to

argument by analogy
is sometimes used.

Crustaceans fulfill several criteria proposed as indicating that non-human animals may experience pain. These fulfilled criteria include a suitable nervous system and sensory receptors; opioid receptors and reduced responses to noxious stimuli when given analgesics and local anaesthetics; physiological changes to noxious stimuli; displaying protective motor reactions; exhibiting avoidance learning; and making trade-offs between noxious stimulus avoidance and other motivational requirements.

In vertebrates, endogenous opioids are neurochemicals that moderate pain by interacting with opioid receptors. Opioid peptides and opioid receptors occur naturally in crustaceans, and although it was concluded in 2005 "at present no certain conclusion can be drawn",[3] more recent considerations suggest their presence along with related physiological and behavioural responses as indicating that crustaceans may experience pain.[4][5] Opioids may moderate pain in crustaceans in a similar way to that in vertebrates. If crustaceans feel pain, there are ethical and animal welfare implications including the consequences of exposure to pollutants, and practices involving commercial and recreational fishing, aquaculture, food preparation and for crustaceans used in scientific research.

Background

The possibility that crustaceans and other non-human animals may experience pain has a long history. Initially, this was based around theoretical and philosophical argument, but more recently has turned to scientific investigation.[citation needed]

Philosophy

Descartes argued that animals could not experience pain

The idea that non-human animals might not feel pain goes back to the 17th-century French philosopher, René Descartes, who argued that animals do not experience pain and suffering because they lack consciousness.[6][7][8] In 1789, the British philosopher and social reformist, Jeremy Bentham, addressed in his book An Introduction to the Principles of Morals and Legislation the issue of our treatment of animals with the following often quoted words: "The question is not, Can they reason? nor, can they talk? but, Can they suffer?"[9]

Peter Singer, a bioethicist and author of Animal Liberation published in 1975, suggested that consciousness is not necessarily the key issue: just because animals have smaller brains, or are ‘less conscious’ than humans, does not mean that they are not capable of feeling pain. He goes on further to argue that we do not assume newborn infants, people suffering from neurodegenerative brain diseases or people with learning disabilities experience less pain than we would.[10]

Bernard Rollin, the principal author of two U.S. federal laws regulating pain relief for animals, writes that researchers remained unsure into the 1980s as to whether animals experience pain, and veterinarians trained in the U.S. before 1989 were taught to simply ignore animal pain.[11] In his interactions with scientists and other veterinarians, Rollin was regularly asked to "prove" that animals are conscious, and to provide "scientifically acceptable" grounds for claiming that they feel pain.[11]

Continuing into the 1990s, discussions were further developed on the roles that philosophy and science had in understanding

amniotes) have at least simple conscious thoughts and feelings[13] and that the view animals feel pain differently to humans is now a minority view.[6]

Scientific investigation

Cambridge Declaration on Consciousness (2012)

The absence of a

neurological substrates that generate consciousness. Non-human animals, including all mammals and birds, and many other creatures, including octopuses, also possess these neurological substrates.[14]

In the 20th and 21st centuries, there were many scientific investigations of pain in non-human animals. Argument by analogy is sometimes used to assess the capacity of other animals to experience pain. This is based on the principle that if a non-human animal's responses to noxious stimuli are similar to those of humans, they are likely to have had an analogous experience. For example, if a pin is stuck in a chimpanzee's finger and it rapidly withdraws its hand, then argument by analogy indicates that like humans, it felt pain.[15][16][17]

In 2012 the American philosopher Gary Varner reviewed the research literature on pain in animals. His findings are summarised in the following table.[18] Arguing by analogy, Varner claims that any animal which exhibits the properties listed in the table could be said to experience pain. On that basis, he concludes that all vertebrates, including fish, probably experience pain, but invertebrates (e.g. crustaceans) apart from cephalopods probably do not experience pain.[18][19]

Vertebrates

Arthritic rats self-select analgesic opiates.[20] In 2014, the veterinary Journal of Small Animal Practice published an article on the recognition of pain which started – "The ability to experience pain is universally shared by all mammals...".[21] Birds with gait abnormalities self-select for a diet that contains carprofen, a human analgesic.[22] In 2005, it was written "Avian pain is likely analogous to pain experienced by most mammals"[23] and in 2014, "it is accepted that birds perceive and respond to noxious stimuli and that birds feel pain."[24] Veterinary articles have been published stating both reptiles[25][26][27] and amphibians[28][29][30] experience pain in a way analogous to humans, and that analgesics are effective in these two classes of vertebrates.

Argument by analogy[18]
Property Invertebrates Vertebrates
Earth­worms Insects Leeches/​snails Ceph­alopods Fish Amphib­ians Reptiles Birds Mamm­als
Has nociceptors ? Red XN Green tickY ? Green tickY Green tickY Green tickY Green tickY Green tickY
Has brain Red XN Red XN Red XN Green tickY Green tickY Green tickY Green tickY Green tickY Green tickY
Nociceptors and brain linked Red XN Red XN Red XN Green tickY Green tickY ? / Green tickY ? / Green tickY ? / Green tickY Green tickY
Has
endogenous opioids
Green tickY Green tickY ? Red XN Green tickY Green tickY Green tickY Green tickY Green tickY
Analgesics affect responses ? ? ? ? Green tickY ? ? Green tickY Green tickY
Response to damaging stimuli similar to humans Red XN Red XN ? Green tickY Green tickY Green tickY Green tickY Green tickY Green tickY

In the table -

Green tickY denotes Varner believes reliable research indicates the taxon has the attribute
Red XN denotes Varner believes reliable research indicates the taxon does not have the attribute
? denotes Varner believes reliable research has not ascertained whether the taxon has the attribute or varies between species within the taxa.

Adaptive value

The

maladaptive. It is often suggested hyperalgesia and allodynia assist organisms to protect themselves during healing, but experimental evidence to support this has been lacking.[31][32]

In 2014, the adaptive value of sensitisation due to injury was tested using the predatory interactions between longfin inshore squid (Doryteuthis pealeii) and black sea bass (Centropristis striata) which are natural predators of this squid. If injured squid are targeted by a bass, they began their defensive behaviours sooner (indicated by greater alert distances and longer flight initiation distances) than uninjured squid. If anaesthetic (1% ethanol and MgCl2) is administered prior to the injury, this prevents the sensitisation and blocks the behavioural effect. The authors claim this study is the first experimental evidence to support the argument that nociceptive sensitisation is actually an adaptive response to injuries.[33]

The experience of pain

Although there are numerous definitions of pain, almost all involve two key components.

First, nociception is required.[34] This is the ability to detect noxious stimuli which evoke a reflex response that rapidly moves the entire animal, or the affected part of its body, away from the source of the stimulus. The concept of nociception does not imply any adverse, subjective "feeling" – it is a reflex action. An example in humans would be the rapid withdrawal of a finger that has touched something hot – the withdrawal occurs before any sensation of pain is actually experienced.[citation needed]

The second component is the experience of "pain" itself, or suffering – the internal, emotional interpretation of the nociceptive experience. Again in humans, this is when the withdrawn finger begins to hurt, moments after the withdrawal. Pain is therefore a private, emotional experience. Nociceptive reflexes act to immediately remove the animal or part of the body from a (potentially) damaging stimulus. However, without learning from this experience, the animal would likely expose itself to the damaging stimulus repeatedly. Pain has the adaptive advantage that it invokes a level of learning, thereby preventing the animal from repeatedly exposing itself to potential injury.[35] Pain cannot be directly measured in other animals, including other humans; responses to putatively painful stimuli can be measured, but not the experience itself. To address this problem when assessing the capacity of other species to experience pain, argument-by-analogy is used. This is based on the principle that if an animal responds to a stimulus in a similar way to ourselves, it is likely to have had an analogous experience.[citation needed]

Nociception

Nociception: The reflex arc of a dog with a pin in her paw. Note there is no communication to the brain, but the paw is withdrawn by nervous impulses generated by the spinal cord. There is no conscious interpretation of the stimulus by the dog involved in the reflex itself.

In vertebrates, nociceptive responses involve the transmission of a signal along a chain of

nerve fibres from the site of a noxious stimulus at the periphery, to the spinal cord. This process evokes a reflex arc response such as flinching or immediate withdrawal of a limb, generated at the spinal cord and not involving the brain. Nociception is found, in one form or another, across all major animal taxa.[34] Nociception can be observed using modern imaging techniques and both physiological and behavioural responses to nociception can be detected. Many crustacean species, including the rockpool prawn (Palaemon elegans),[36] exhibit the caridoid escape reaction – an immediate, nociceptive, reflex tail-flick response to noxious stimuli (see here[37]
).

Emotional pain

Sometimes a distinction is made between "physical pain" and "emotional" or "psychological pain". Emotional pain is the pain experienced in the absence of physical trauma, e.g. the pain experienced by humans after the loss of a loved one, or the break-up of a relationship. It has been argued that only primates, including humans, can feel "emotional pain". However, research has provided evidence that monkeys, dogs, cats and birds can show signs of emotional pain and display behaviours associated with depression during painful experience, i.e. lack of motivation, lethargy, anorexia, unresponsiveness to other animals.[10]

Physical pain

The nerve impulses of the nociception response may be conducted to the brain thereby registering the location, intensity, quality and unpleasantness of the stimulus. This subjective component of pain involves conscious awareness of both the sensation and the unpleasantness (the aversive, negative affect). The brain processes underlying conscious awareness of the unpleasantness (suffering), are not well understood.

There have been several published lists of criteria for establishing whether non-human animals experience pain, e.g.[38][39] Some criteria that may indicate the potential of another species, including crustaceans, to feel pain include:[39]

  1. Has a suitable
    sensory receptors
  2. Has
    local anaesthetics
  3. Physiological changes to
    noxious stimuli
  4. Displays protective motor reactions that might include reduced use of an affected area such as limping, rubbing, holding or autotomy
  5. Shows avoidance learning
  6. Shows trade-offs between noxious stimulus avoidance and other motivational requirements
  7. High
    cognitive ability and sentience

Research findings

A swimming prawn showing buoyancy in its natural environment

The vast majority of research on pain in crustaceans has used (semi-) aquatic,

selection pressures on different animal groups, as well as exposing them to differing types of nociceptive stimuli. For example, crustaceans living in an aquatic world can maintain a certain level of buoyancy, so the risk of collision due to gravity is limited compared with a terrestrial vertebrate. Similarly, noxious chemicals might be diluted considerably in an aquatic environment compared to terrestrial. Therefore, nociceptive and pain systems in aquatic animals may be quite dissimilar to terrestrial animals.[40]

Peripheral nervous system

The nervous systems of a selection of crustaceans.

Receptors

Crayfish (Procambarus clarkii) respond quickly and strongly to high temperatures, however, they show no response to low temperature stimuli, or, when stimulated with capsaicin or isothiocyanate (both are irritants to mammals). Noxious high temperatures are considered to be a potentially ecologically relevant noxious stimulus for crayfish that can be detected by sensory neurons, which may be specialized nociceptors.[41]

The common brown shrimp

acclimation temperature.[42]

Nerve fibres

Crayfish have peripheral nerve fibres[43] which are responsive to noxious stimuli.[41]

Neurons functionally specialized for nociception have been documented in other invertebrates including the leech

Aplysia californica and Cepaea nemoralis. Changes in neuronal activity induced by noxious stimuli have been recorded in the nervous centres of Caenorhabditis elegans, Drosophila melanogaster and larval Manduca sexta.[4]

Central nervous system

The bodies of crustaceans are segmented; there is one

swimmerets and mouthparts. The ganglia show great functional autonomy; information received by the ganglion is processed by the same ganglion, enabling a faster response than if the message had to travel all the way up the animal's body to the brain and then back. Information can be exchanged between ganglia enabling the animal to perform coordinated movements.[citation needed
]

When shore crabs (

cheliped (claw), this evokes specific nociceptive behavior and neurochemical responses in the thoracic ganglia and the brain.[4]

Brain

protocerebrum, which consists of two optic lobes, and the median protocerebrum.[44]

In 2002, James Rose (University of Wyoming) and more recently Brian Key (University of Queensland) published reviews arguing that fish (and presumably crustaceans) cannot feel pain because they lack a

subcortical brain networks.[14]

Opioid system and effects of analgesics

Opiates modulate nociception in vertebrates. In vertebrates, morphine is an analgesic and therefore ameliorates the sensation of pain. Naloxone is an opioid-receptor antagonist and therefore blocks the effects of morphine. Opioid modulation of nociception has been demonstrated in several invertebrate species.[49]

A mantis shrimp swimming in its natural environment. The first report of opiate effects in invertebrates is based on a mantis shrimp species

The first report of opiate effects in invertebrates is based on the behavioural responses of the crustacean mantis shrimp Squilla mantis. These shrimp respond to an electric shock with an immediate, violent, convulsive-like flexion of the body. If they are injected with morphine-HCL, this produces a dose-dependent analgesia by increasing the intensity threshold to the shock. This effect is fully blocked by naloxone.[50]

Crustaceans have a functional opioid system which includes the presence of opioid receptors similar to those of mammals.

RT-PCR research on the American lobster (Homarus americanus) has revealed the presence of a Mu-opioid receptor transcript in neural and immune tissues, which exhibits a 100% sequence identity with its human counterpart.[52]

In the American lobster, endogenous morphine is found in the

pereiopod (walking leg) cut off or been injected with the irritant lipopolysaccharide, the endogenous morphine levels initially increased by 24% for haemolymph and 48% for the nerve cord.[52]

In vertebrates, opioid peptides (i.e., enkephalins) have been shown to be involved in nociception. Leu-enkephalin and Met-enkephalin are present in the thoracic ganglia of the shore crab, Carcinus maenas.[53]

Both morphine and naloxone affect the estuarine crab (Neohelice granulata) in a similar way to their effects on vertebrates: injections of morphine produce a dose-dependent reduction of their defensive response to an electric shock.[54] However, it has been suggested the attenuated defensive response could originate from either the analgesic or sedative properties of morphine, or both.[55] One study on the effects of a danger stimulus on the crab Chasmagnathus granulatus reported this induces opioid analgesia, which is influenced by naloxone.[56] In American lobsters, the response of endogenous morphine in both haemocytes and neural cells to noxious stimuli are mediated by naloxone.[52]

When the antennae of rockpool prawns Palaemon elegans are rubbed with sodium hydroxide or acetic acid (both are irritants in mammals), they increase grooming and rubbing of the afflicted area against the side of the tank; this reaction is inhibited by benzocaine (a local anaesthetic in mammals), even though control prawns treated with only anaesthetic do not show reduced activity.[36]

The whiteleg shrimp (juvenile shown) has become the preferred species for shrimp farming

lignocaine (a local anaesthetic in mammals), showed less rubbing, flicking and sheltering than those without the anaesthetic.[58]

One study on reducing the stress of prawns resulting from transportation concluded that Aqui-STM and

clove oil (a natural anaesthetic) may be suitable anaesthetic treatments for prawns.[59]

Physiological responses

Higher levels of stress, as measured by lactate, occur in shore crabs exposed to brief electric shock compared to non-shocked controls. However, shocked crabs showed more vigorous behaviour than controls, possibly indicating it is increased behaviour causing the increased lactate. But, when crabs with the same level of behaviour are matched, shocked crabs still have a stronger stress response compared with controls. The authors suggested that their findings, coupled with previous findings of long-term motivational change and avoidance learning, "fulfils the criteria expected of a pain experience".[5] Others have criticised these findings, including the fact that the lactate levels measured were within the normal range measured for shore crabs, and that any increases in lactate in shocked crabs were possibly due to increased anaerobic activity. They also argued that behavioural "activities that go beyond mere reflex responses" is an inadequate criterion for pain.[60]

In crayfish (Procambarus clarkii), anxiolytic (stress-reducing) drugs made for humans also reduce anxiety.[61]

Injection of formalin into the

cheliped of shore crabs (Hemigrapsus sanguineus) evokes specific nociceptive behavior and neurochemical responses in the brain and thoracic ganglion.[62]

Protective responses

A hermit crab outside of its shell. Note the soft, curved abdomen which is vulnerable to predators.

Most species of hermit crab have long, spirally curved abdomens, which are soft, unlike the hard, calcified abdomens seen in related crustaceans. They protect themselves from predators by entering a salvaged empty seashell, into which they can retract their whole body. As they grow, they must leave their shell and find another larger, more suitable shell.[63] Their shells are therefore highly valuable to them. When hermit crabs (Pagurus bernhardus) are given an electric shock, they leave their shells and subsequently perform prolonged abdominal grooming at the site of where they received the shock.[64]

Male Chasmagnathus granulatus crabs exhibit a "defensive response" to electric shocks.

formalin (an irritant in mammals) or saline into one cheliped (the leg which ends with the claw), shore crabs move quickly into the corner of the aquarium and "freeze" after 2 to 3 seconds. After 1 to 3 minutes, these injected animals are fidgety and exhibit a wide range of movements such as flexion, extension, shaking or rubbing the affected claw. Formalin-treated animals show 20-times more rubbing behaviour during the first minute after injection than saline-treated crabs. Intense rubbing of the claw results in autotomy (shedding) in 20% of animals of the formalin-treated group whereas saline-injected crabs do not autotomise the injected cheliped. During the 10 minutes after injection, crabs in the formalin-treated group tried to use the intact cheliped, guarding the damaged cheliped. The scientists conducting this study commented "the present results obtained in crabs may be indicative of pain experience rather than relating to a simple nociceptive reflex".[4] Other species of crab have been shown to autotomise legs when placed on a hot plate or exposed to a small electric shock.[15]

When the antennae of rockpool prawns (Palaemon elegans) are rubbed with sodium hydroxide or acetic acid (both are irritants), the animals show increased grooming and rubbing of the afflicted area against the side of the tank. Furthermore, this reaction is inhibited by a local anaesthetic, even though control prawns treated with only anaesthetic did not show reduced activity. Other scientists suggested the rubbing may reflect an attempt to clean the affected area[66] as application of anaesthetic alone caused an increase in grooming.

In one study, no behavioural or neural changes in three different crustacean species (red swamp crayfish (

Palaemonetes sp.) were observed in response to noxious acids or bases.[67]

Avoidance learning

Shore crabs quickly (within 1 or 2 trials) learn to avoid one of two dark shelters if choosing that shelter consistently results in them receiving an electric shock.[68] The crayfish Procambarus clarkii and the crab Chasmagnathus granulatus learn to associate an electric shock with a light turning on, or with the occupancy of the light compartment of the aquarium, respectively. They quickly learn to respond to these associations by walking to a safe area in which the shock is not delivered (crayfish) or by refraining from entering the light compartment (crab).[15]

Trade-offs in motivation

A hermit crab

Nociceptive responses are reflexes that do not change regardless of motivational priorities. In contrast, a painful experience may change the motivation for normal behavioural responses, thereby indicating a plastic response to an aversive stimulus, rather than a simple reflex response.[39]

In 2009, Elwood and Mirjam Appel showed that hermit crabs make motivational trade-offs between electric shocks and the quality of the shells they inhabit.[69] In particular, as hermit crabs are shocked more intensely, they become increasingly willing to leave their current shells for new shells, and they spend less time deciding whether to enter those new shells. Moreover, because the researchers did not offer the new shells until after the electrical stimulation had ended, the change in motivational behavior was the result of memory of the noxious event, not an immediate reflex. It was also shown that hermit crabs experiencing increasing electric shocks left their shell at a reduced intensity when the shell was from a less preferred species than did those in shells of a more desirable species. This demonstrates that hermit crabs are willing to risk predator attack by evacuating their shells to avoid a noxious stimulus and that this is dependent upon how valuable the shell is.[40]

A similar study shows that hermit crabs are less likely to leave their shells after electric shock if they are in an environment that contains the odour of predators. This shows the crabs trade-off the motivation to avoid electric shocks and predator avoidance.[70]

Shore crabs (Carcinus maenas) also show motivational trade-offs; they will discard a valuable resource (a preferred shelter) to avoid future encounters with painful stimuli, thereby indicating avoidance learning – a key criterion of the ability to experience pain.[65]

A 2014 study[71] on crayfish (Procambarus clarkii) tested their responses in a fear paradigm, the elevated plus maze in which animals choose to walk on an elevated cross which offers both aversive and preferable conditions (in this case, two arms were lit and two were dark). Crayfish which experienced an electric shock displayed enhanced fearfulness or anxiety as demonstrated by their preference for the dark arms more than the light. Furthermore, shocked crayfish had relatively higher brain serotonin concentrations coupled with elevated blood glucose, which suggests a stress response.[40] A follow-up study using the same species showed the intensity of the anxiety-like behaviour, presumably resulting from the pain, was dependent on the intensity of the electric shock until reaching a plateau. Such a quantitative relationship between stress and anxiety is also a very common feature of human and vertebrate anxiety.[72]

Legislation

Legislation protects some invertebrates when they are being used in research; the taxa protected varies between countries and regions.

Legislation on the protection of invertebrates in research adapted from[73]
Country or
region
Invertebrates protected Legislation
Australia
(some states)
Cephalopods Government National Health and Medical
Research Council's Code of Practice (2004)
Canada Cephalopods and "some other higher invertebrates" Canadian Council on Animal Care (1991)[74]
EU Cephalopods
EU Directive 2010/63/EU[75]
New Zealand Octopuses, squids, crabs, lobsters, crayfish Animal Welfare Act (1999)[76]
Norway Squids, octopuses, decapod crustaceans, honeybees Norwegian Animal Welfare Act (2009)[77]
Switzerland Cephalopods, decapod crustaceans Swiss Animal Welfare Act (2008)[78]

Jonathan Birch, philosopher of biology at the London School of Economics, argue we should apply the precautionary principle to animal sentience. Without lowering scientific standards, this principle leads to include in our animal protection laws any species for which we have at least a credible indicator of sentience. If we have good reasons to believe that a species of crab is sentient, it is sufficient to believe that all crabs (more than 4000 species) are sentient. Therefore, it is all the members of the decapod order that should be included in our animal welfare laws.[79]

Opinions

Advocates for Animals, a Scottish animal welfare group, stated in 2005 that "scientific evidence ... strongly suggests that there is a potential for decapod crustaceans and cephalopods to experience pain and suffering". This is primarily due to "The likelihood that decapod crustaceans can feel pain [which] is supported by the fact that they have been shown to have opioid receptors and to respond to opioids (analgesics such as morphine) in a similar way to vertebrates." Similarities between decapod and vertebrate stress systems and behavioral responses to noxious stimuli were given as additional evidence for the capacity of decapods to experience pain.[80]

In 2005 a review of the literature by the Norwegian Scientific Committee for Food Safety tentatively concluded that "it is unlikely that [lobsters] can feel pain," though they note that "there is apparently a paucity of exact knowledge on sentience in crustaceans, and more research is needed." This conclusion is based on the lobster's simple nervous system. The report assumes that the violent reaction of lobsters to boiling water is a reflex response (i.e. does not involve conscious perception) to noxious stimuli.[3]

A European Food Safety Authority (EFSA) 2005 publication[81] stated that the largest of decapod crustaceans have complex behaviour, a pain system, considerable learning abilities and appear to have some degree of awareness. Based on this evidence, they placed all decapod crustaceans into the same category of research-animal protection as vertebrates.

Pain during killing

This tanner crab was cut in half before cooking

The EFSA summarized that the killing methods most likely to cause pain and distress are:[82]

  • Any procedures whereby the abdomen is separated from the thorax
  • The removal of tissue, flesh, or limbs while the crustacean is alive and fully conscious
  • Placing crustaceans in water slowly heated to the boiling point
  • Placing crustaceans directly into boiling water
  • Placing marine crustaceans in fresh water
  • Unfocused microwaving of the body as opposed to focal application to the head

A device called the

crabs, and crayfish before cooking. The device works by applying a 120-volt, 2 to 5 amp electrical charge to the animal. The CrustaStun renders the shellfish unconscious in 0.3 seconds and kills the animal in 5 to 10 seconds, compared to 3 minutes to kill a lobster by boiling.[83]

See also

References

  1. S2CID 35448280.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link
    )
  2. ^ .
  3. ^ a b Sømme, L. (2005). "Sentience and pain in invertebrates: Report to Norwegian Scientific Committee for Food Safety". Norwegian University of Life Sciences, Oslo.
  4. ^
    PMID 22786644.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link
    )
  5. ^ .
  6. ^ .
  7. ^ Radner, D. & Radner, M. (1989). Animal Consciousness. Prometheus Books: Buffalo.
  8. JSTOR 2220217
    .
  9. ^ "Bentham, J. (1879). An Introduction to the Principles of Morals and Legislation. Clarendon Press.
  10. ^ a b Sneddon, L.U. "Can animals feel pain?". The Welcome Trust. Archived from the original on April 13, 2012. Retrieved September 24, 2015.
  11. ^ a b Rollin, B. (1989). The Unheeded Cry: Animal Consciousness, Animal Pain, and Science. Oxford University Press, pp. xii, 117–118, cited in Carbone 2004, p. 150.
  12. PMID 9464883. Archived from the original
    on 2015-09-29. Retrieved 2016-02-23.
  13. .
  14. ^ a b Low, P. (July 7, 2012). Jaak Panksepp; Diana Reiss; David Edelman; Bruno Van Swinderen; Philip Low; Christof Koch (eds.). "The Cambridge declaration on consciousness" (PDF). University of Cambridge. Archived (PDF) from the original on October 9, 2022. Retrieved February 23, 2016.
  15. ^
    PMID 20061665
    .
  16. .
  17. PMID 21709310. Archived from the original
    (PDF) on 2012-04-07.
  18. ^ from the original on 2024-01-23. Retrieved 2020-11-07. Table 5.2, page 113.
  19. from the original on 2024-01-23. Retrieved 2020-11-07.
  20. S2CID 24858615.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link
    )
  21. PMID 24841489.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link
    )
  22. S2CID 35062797.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link
    )
  23. .
  24. ^ Paul-Murphy, J. & Hawkins, M.G. (2014). "Chapter 26 - Bird-specific considerations: recognizing pain in pet birds.". In Gaynor, J.S. & Muir III, W. W. (eds.). Handbook of Veterinary Pain Management. Elsevier Health Sciences.
  25. .
  26. .
  27. .
  28. .
  29. .
  30. .
  31. .
  32. ^ "Maladaptive pain". Oxford Reference. Archived from the original on July 4, 2015. Retrieved May 16, 2016.
  33. PMID 24814149.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link
    )
  34. ^ from the original on 2019-08-03. Retrieved 2019-08-03.
  35. .
  36. ^
    S2CID 53203416.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link
    )
  37. ^ "Crayfish video - tail-flip Response - neur3010". YouTube. Archived from the original on 2017-02-24. Retrieved 2016-02-28.
  38. S2CID 53194458. Archived from the original on 2015-12-23. Retrieved 2016-02-23.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link
    )
  39. ^
    doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2009.02.018.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link
    )
  40. ^ .
  41. ^ .
  42. ^ Davies, C.A. (1981). Thermal sensitivity and behaviour in the brown shrimp and some related crustacea (Doctoral dissertation) (Thesis). University College of Swansea.
  43. ^ "Behavioral Neuroscience, Crayfish Circuitry". Archived from the original on 2016-03-04. Retrieved 2016-02-25.
  44. ^ Sandeman, D. (1990). "Structural and functional levels in the organization of decapod crustacean brains". Frontiers in Crustacean Neurobiology. Birkhäuser Basel. pp. 223–239.
  45. S2CID 16220451. Archived from the original
    (PDF) on 2012-10-10.
  46. ^ Rose, J.D. (2002). "Do fish feel pain?". Archived from the original on January 20, 2013. Retrieved September 27, 2007.
  47. .
  48. ^ Sneddon, L.U. (2012). "Pain perception in fish: Evidence and implications for the use of fish". Journal of Consciousness Studies. 18: 209–229.
  49. PMID 20397037.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link
    )
  50. .
  51. S2CID 26232087.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link
    )
  52. ^
    PMID 15855878.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link
    )
  53. PMID 11607217.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link
    )
  54. ^
    S2CID 45083722.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link
    )
  55. .
  56. S2CID 5550319.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link
    )
  57. .
  58. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2012.06.002.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link
    )
  59. doi:10.1111/j.1749-7345.2005.tb00332.x.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link
    )
  60. (PDF) from the original on 2016-06-05. Retrieved 2016-04-25.
  61. .
  62. .
  63. ^ "Incredible footage of hermit crab changing shells with anemones!". YouTube. Archived from the original on 2016-04-05. Retrieved 2016-02-28.
  64. S2CID 54366021
    .
  65. ^ (PDF) from the original on 2019-09-02. Retrieved 2019-09-02.
  66. ^ Ian Sample (November 8, 2007). "Blow for fans of boiled lobster: crustaceans feel pain, study says". The Guardian. Archived from the original on October 21, 2016. Retrieved December 16, 2016.
  67. PMID 20422026
    .
  68. .
  69. S2CID 53197401. Archived from the original
    (PDF) on December 15, 2010.
  70. .
  71. S2CID 43094402.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link
    )
  72. PMID 26139659.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link
    )
  73. ^ Tonkins, B.B.M. (2016). Why are cephalopods protected in scientific research in Europe?
  74. ^ "Guide To The care and Use Of Experimental Animals" (PDF). Canadian Council on Animal Care. Archived (PDF) from the original on May 19, 2017. Retrieved December 24, 2016.
  75. ^ "Directive 2010/63/EU of the European Parliament and of the Council". Official Journal of the European Union. Archived from the original on July 21, 2018. Retrieved December 23, 2016.
  76. ^ "Animal Welfare Act (1999)". New Zealand legislation. Archived from the original on October 19, 2016. Retrieved December 23, 2016.
  77. ^ "Norwegian Animal Welfare Act". Michigan State University. Archived from the original on February 1, 2016. Retrieved December 23, 2016.
  78. ^ "see the section "Animal Welfare Ordinance"". Archived from the original on December 24, 2016. Retrieved December 23, 2016.
  79. ^ Birch, Jonathan (2017) Animal sentience and the precautionary principle. Animal Sentience 16(1)
  80. ^ Cephalopods and decapod crustaceans: their capacity to experience pain and suffering (PDF). Advocates for Animals. 2005. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2012-05-14. Retrieved 2011-11-23.
  81. ^ "Opinion on the aspects of the biology and welfare of animals used for experimental and other scientific purposes". The EFSA Journal. 292: 1–46. 2005.
  82. ^ Yue, S. (2008). "The welfare of crustaceans at slaughter". Impacts on Farm Animals. Archived from the original on 2016-06-17. Retrieved 2016-02-23.
  83. ^ McSmith, A. (2009). "I'll have my lobster electrocuted, please". The Independent (Newspaper). Archived from the original on 2022-05-24. Retrieved June 14, 2013.

Further reading